U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

National Collaborating Centre for Mental Health (UK). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Diagnosis and Management of ADHD in Children, Young People and Adults. Leicester (UK): British Psychological Society (UK); 2009. (NICE Clinical Guidelines, No. 72.)

  • March 2018: NICE has made new recommendations on recognition, information and support, managing ADHD (including non-pharmacological treatment), medication, monitoring, adherence, and review of medication and discontinuation. The recommendations and evidence in chapters 4, 7, 8, 10, 11 and 12 have been stood down and replaced. They are marked with grey shading in the PDF. February 2016: NICE has made new recommendations on dietary interventions and dietary advice, which can be found in the Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis and management update (CG72.1). The recommendations and evidence in chapter 9 & section 12.4.2 of this guideline that have been highlighted in grey in the PDF have been stood down and replaced.

March 2018: NICE has made new recommendations on recognition, information and support, managing ADHD (including non-pharmacological treatment), medication, monitoring, adherence, and review of medication and discontinuation. The recommendations and evidence in chapters 4, 7, 8, 10, 11 and 12 have been stood down and replaced. They are marked with grey shading in the PDF. February 2016: NICE has made new recommendations on dietary interventions and dietary advice, which can be found in the Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis and management update (CG72.1). The recommendations and evidence in chapter 9 & section 12.4.2 of this guideline that have been highlighted in grey in the PDF have been stood down and replaced.

Cover of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: Diagnosis and Management of ADHD in Children, Young People and Adults.

Show details

APPENDIX 16ADHD CONSENSUS CONFERENCE

PART 1. SUMMARIES OF PRESENTATIONS PROVIDED BY THE ADHD CONSENSUS CONFERENCE SPEAKERS, 17 OCTOBER 2006

1.1. THE VALUE AND LIMITATIONS OF THE CONCEPTS OF ADHD AND HYPERKINETIC DISORDER IN GUIDING TREATMENT - A CLINICIAN’S PERSPECTIVE

Authors

1.

Affiliations

1 Senior Lecturer in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Division of Pathology and Neuroscience (Psychiatry), University of Dundee

The presentation will look at the value and limitation of ADHD as a concept and develop ideas by looking at the questions posed in the outline of the position statement.

1.1.1. To what extent do the phenomena of overactivity, inattentiveness and impulsiveness cluster into a particular disorder that can be distinguished from others and from normal variation?

Internal validity

Inattention, overactivity and impulsivity are all continuous variables which appear to be complex characteristics distributed throughout the population with a fairly normal distribution; these are normally distributed characteristics which therefore blend into the normal. The distinction of what is and what is not normal has to be, by definition, arbitrary.

Factor analysis suggests that their distribution is not random but shows a strong coherence with each other and far less coherence with behaviours characteristic of other conditions such as phobia aggression or anxiety.

1.1.2. At what level, and in what circumstances, do these become impairing for the person?

To some extent where to draw the line as to when symptoms and behaviour are impairing is arbitrary, as it is on a continuum. Symptoms must be related to impairment.

The key issue is, how do symptoms relate to impairment? Impairment can be measured in several ways; however the Children’s Global Assessment Scale (C-GAS) provides a relatively simple and valid measure, scored from 0–100 with 0 indicating the most severe impairment and 100 the most healthy and well-functioning child.

DSM-IV field trials used a C-GAS score of ≤60 (which implies impairment requiring specific treatment) and determined that five ADHD symptoms were required to be present to reach this cut-off. To avoid false positives the number was increased to six or more symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity/impulsivity.

Problems occur with children whose impairment arising from their ADHD symptoms is really quite severe, but who technically do not meet the diagnostic criteria.

1.1.3. Impact of ADHD on overall functioning

An important part of impairment arising from ADHD is its breadth, including impact on:

  • social, academic and interpersonal domains
  • the family
  • ideas of self-worth.

What is particularly interesting to a clinician is how to reduce the functional impairments consequent to these symptoms and any comorbidities.

1.1.4. Impact of untreated and under-treated ADHD

Apart from the impact on the individual, ADHD has an impact on the following areas:

  • The healthcare system: 50% increase in bike accidents, 33% increase in emergency room visits, 2 to 4 times more vehicle accidents.
  • School and occupation: 46% expelled, 35% drop out and lower occupational status.
  • Family: 3 to 5 times increase in parental divorce or separation and 2 to 4 times increase in sibling fights.
  • Employer: increase in parental absenteeism and decrease in productivity.
  • Society: twice the risk of substance misuse, earlier onset and individuals are less likely to quit in adulthood.

Children with ADHD are in the bottom 5% of children in terms of quality of life.

1.1.5. The clinical picture for the individual

The symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity combined with a number of psychiatric coexisting conditions such as oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder lead to a number of psychosocial impairments across a number of domains: self, school (or work), home and social.

1.1.6. Is there is evidence for a characteristic pattern of developmental changes, or outcome(s)?

ADHD symptoms were designed for primary school children, and an adult with ADHD is a child with ADHD who has grown up but continues to have problems. The symptoms experienced by these groups will differ; the levels of symptoms and impairment may not necessarily change at the same rate. Although individuals may have symptoms throughout their life they may not demonstrate impairment until later in life.

In children the pattern of symptoms/behaviours is characterised by motor hyper-activity, aggressiveness, low levels of tolerance, impulsiveness and being easily distracted. In adults the pattern is characterised by: inattentiveness, shifting activities, being easily bored, impatience and restlessness.

There is a characteristic pattern of developmental changes. During the pre-school years the child may show some level of behavioural disturbance. Once at school, academic, social and self-esteem problems begin to manifest themselves. As an adolescent, additional issues surrounding smoking and injury begin to appear and by the time the individual is of college age, a pattern of academic failure, occupational difficulties, substance misuse, injury and low self-esteem is apparent. As an adult, relationship problems will also occur.

1.1.7. Is there a specific response to clinical, educational and/or other interventions?

Home-and school-based behavioural treatments and treatment with methylphenidate, dexamfetamine, atomoxetine and several other drugs reduce symptoms and improve functioning. Treatment with other psychoactive medications such as SSRIs or antipsychotics does not have the same effect.

1.1.8. Is there evidence for a consistent heritability, neurobiological or other causality?

We do not know the cause of ADHD. Given that the causes are multifactorial, leading to a common behavioural phenotype, to search for a cause is probably not going to bear fruit.

ADHD aggregates in families with three to five times increased risk in first-degree relatives; twin studies suggest considerable heritability with between 65 and 90% of the phenotypic variance explained by genetic factors. There are also associations with a range of environmental risks (mainly non-shared factors) such as pre- and perinatal complications, low birth weight, prenatal exposure to benzodiazepines, alcohol and nicotine, and brain diseases and injuries.

Gene-environment interactions are likely to play a significant part. Genetic variations cause functional abnormalities in both dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurotransmission within frontostriatal pathways. This in turn leads to deficits in executive and reward-related functioning and subsequently the behavioural manifestations of ADHD.

Finally in terms of response to medication, children with ADHD performed as poorly on a memory task as elderly people with Alzheimer’s disease, and reverted back to normal with one dose of medication.

1.1.9. The value of the concept of ADHD

  • Reliability and validity is well established.
  • It defines a group of children with considerable impairment.

    It also defines those with symptoms but no impairment.

  • These impairments affect not only the person with the diagnosis but also their family and community.
  • It defines a group that has a high risk of suffering from a wide range of other difficulties.
  • It provides a starting point from and an anchor on which clinicians can base their assessments.
  • It defines a group which responds to (and will benefit from) treatment.
  • It defines a group with a disability that is currently under-recognised and under-treated in the UK.
  • It defines a group whose numbers are relatively stable across time and across cultures.
  • It does not assume pathophysiology where this is not warranted but has strong associations with a range of biological measures for example, heritability, pathophysiology and neuropsychology.
  • The diagnoses are now almost universally used in research studies into the causes, associations and treatment of ADHD. This provides a strong link between scientific research and clinical practice.

1.1.10. The limitations of the concept of ADHD

  • It can lead to dispute and misunderstanding concerning the ‘correct’ system.
  • Categorical definition of a dimensional concept.

    Cut offs are arbitrary with a big impact on prevalence.

  • Inattentiveness symptoms are not adequately defined.
  • It defines a heterogeneous group.
  • It can be misused if impairment is not adequately considered.
  • The exclusion of comorbid forms within the ICD-10 criteria is not helpful when that is the picture of the case in front of you.
  • It can lead to difficulties in identifying those requiring treatment.

    For example, those with subthreshold symptoms but considerable impairment.

  • Research has tended to focus on pure ADHD with much less information on those with comorbidity.
  • Research has tended to concentrate on reduction in core symptoms rather than on the broader outcomes of impairment, quality of life and comorbidity.
  • Neither is adequate for understanding pre-school or adult populations and have limitations with respect to adolescents.

1.2. THE CASE FOR WIDER RECOGNITION OF ADHD – FROM A PAEDIATRIC PERSPECTIVE

Authors

1.

Affiliations

1 Consultant Paediatrician/Physician, Director of Learning Assessment and Neurocare Centre, Horsham, West Sussex

1.2.1. Previous significant under-recognition of ADHD

As noted with concern by the author in 1998 (Kewley, 1998)60 there was continuing under-recognition of ADHD, because of i) persistent reliance on the ICD-10 hyperkinetic terminology; ii) psychosocial-only causes were seen as being solely responsible for all children’s behavioural problems; iii) the copious myth and misinformation and the professional and societal ignorance about ADHD, its nature and complications persisted; and iv) there were divisions between professional groups, fixed professional beliefs, theoretical standpoints and a tendency to debate over the heads of the sufferers. Despite the fact that ADHD was the most referenced childhood condition in the Index Medicus during the 1970s and 1980s (Cantwell, 1996), the above difficulties had meant that ADHD was not validated in the UK until the NICE report of 2000 (Lord & Paisley, 2000), was significantly under-recognised and was very slow to be considered as part of the provision of effective child, adolescent and adult mental health services. Although since 2000 there has been an improvement in recognition and validity of ADHD, all of the above problems persist and affect the recognition and provision of effective children’s mental health services today. Clinical experience and review of international literature concluded that DSM-IV-R had been a much more effective way of providing effective services. The NICE (2000) report noted that medication usage, however, is but one means of reflecting the increased recognition and diagnosis of ADHD.

1.2.2. Guidelines

It was noted that over the past 8 years there has been a degree of convergence between the DSM-IV-R and hyperkinetic (ICD-10) approaches to the diagnosis of ADHD (Swanson et al., 1998). The publications of the Eunithydes Group (Banachewski et al., 2006; Taylor et al., 2004) in recent years have led to a much more clinically relevant evidence-based approach. In clinical practice it has become increasingly realistic to use European guidelines to guide patient management. Previously such guidelines had been more theoretical than practical and clinicians had tended to rely more on North American guidelines, such as the Texas Algorithms (Pliszka et al., 2000) and those from the American Association of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Recent European guidelines have been increasingly relevant in guiding audit and helping manage patient care. However, there remains a need for guidelines for complex case management and for working between professional groups such as the youth justice system, social workers, substance misuse services, and so on. It was also clear that both paediatricians and child psychiatrists had a role in managing children and adolescents with ADHD.

1.2.3. Professional and societal recognition of the progression and life span issues of ADHD – relevance to guideline development

Many international studies have emphasised the long-term difficulties of having untreated ADHD (see Figure 1) and the need for differing professional bodies to work together. For example the British Cohort Study (Brassett-Grundy & Butler, 2004) in a 30-year prospective longitudinal study showed that people with childhood ADHD were significantly more likely to face a wide range of negative outcomes at age 30, spanning domains of education, economic status, housing, relationships, crime and health and that their adult lives were typified by social deprivation and adversity. This British study reflects a number of international studies.

Figure 1. Likely progression of children with untreated ADHD.

Figure 1

Likely progression of children with untreated ADHD. Key: ODD = oppositional defiant disorder; MVA = motor vehicle accidents

Such long-term studies confirm the vulnerability created by ADHD. They emphasise the need for wider recognition of ADHD in relation to criminal behaviour, school under-achievement and exclusion, special schooling provision, workplace issues, teenage pregnancies, motor vehicle accidents and gambling. Another related issue is that many older people, who were educated before the recognition of ADHD as a valid condition, still have ongoing, impairing symptomatology as older adolescents or adults.

One such subgroup of particular concern comprises those with long-term difficulties arising from ADHD and related difficulties who have entered the youth justice system. The risk factors for such youths are having ADHD with associated early onset of disruptive behaviour disorder, substance use disorder and/or bipolar disorder.

There are many studies in the criminology literature, which tend to run in parallel to ADHD literature (Farrington, 1996; Moffitt et al., 1996). For example the UK National Epidemiologic Study in 1999 (Stephenson & Goodman, 2001) showed that 6% of 5- to 10-year-old boys have conduct disorder, a high percentage of whom entered the youth justice system. Other studies show that up to 90% of those with early conduct disorder have coexisting ADHD (McArdle et al., 1995). Studies raise the possibility of effective medical treatment as part of an overall package of help. Many studies also show a significantly high incidence of ADHD in the juvenile offender population (Rosler et al., 2004). It would be helpful for guidelines to be established, not only with the medical profession but also with other professions such as the Youth Justice Board, social services, tertiary education, teenage pregnancy initiatives and so on.

Approximately 200,000 youths enter the youth justice system annually (The Home Office, 2003). The Asset-Young offender assessment profile showed that up to 75% of such youths considered themselves to be excessively impulsive (Youth Justice Board, 2006). Studies have also shown that re-offending rates can be reduced from approximately 60 to 10% with effective multimodal management including management of ADHD (McCallon, 2000). There is a strong case to be made for guidelines within education and the healthcare profession that link much better with the youth justice and substance misuse services. Consideration is currently being given to whether or not responsibility for such youths could be with education and health rather than primarily with the Home Office and Youth Justice Board (Allen, 2006).

1.2.4. Summary

Despite greatly improved recognition of ADHD in recent years, it would appear it is still currently under-recognised both in terms of incidence, treatment and effective management, especially if DSM-IV-R criteria are to be used.

Paediatricians and child psychiatrists have a part to play in the diagnosis and management of the condition, as do many other professional groups.

Guidelines for the management of adult ADHD should also be developed.

Future guidelines, if they are to be more representative of children’s mental health issues, and of the progression of ADHD, should be developed not only for the medical profession, as per the NICE guidelines, but also in conjunction with other service providers, such as education, youth justice and substance misuse services.

Broader recognition of the reality, the impact on the family, the chronic course and lifespan issues are essential with regard to public policy development, as an issue of social reform and in the development of effective child, adolescent and adult mental health and educational services.

1.2.5 References
  • Allen R. 2006From Punishment To Problem Solving: A New Approach to Children in Trouble London: Centre for Crime and Justice Studies; Available at: http://www​.crimeandjustice.org.uk.
  • Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Paramala S, et al. Long acting medications for hyperkinetic disorders: a systematic review and European treatment guideline. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;15:476–95. [PubMed: 16680409]
  • Brassett-Grundy A, Butler N. Prevalence and Adult Outcomes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Evidence From a 30-Year Prospective Longitudinal Study. London: Bedford Group for Lifecourse and Statistical Studies; 2004. Available at: http://www​.ioe.ac.uk​/bedfordgroup/publications​/ADHD_outcomes_2408.pdf.
  • Cantwell DP. Attention deficit disorder: a review of the past 10 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 1996;35:978–987. [PubMed: 8755794]
  • Farrington D. Understanding and Preventing Youth Crime. York: York Publishing Services in association with the Joseph Rowntree Foundation; 1996.
  • Home Office, The . Youth Justice: The Next Steps. London: The Home Office; 2003.
  • Kewley GD. Personal paper: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder is underdiagnosed and undertreated in Britain. British Medical Journal. 1998;316:1594–1596. [PMC free article: PMC1113204] [PubMed: 9596603]
  • Lord J, Paisley S. The Clinical Effectiveness and Cost-Effectiveness of Methylphenidate for Hyperactivity in Childhood, Version 2. London: National Institute for Clinical Excellence; 2000.
  • McArdle P, O’Brien G, Kolvin I. Hyperactivity: prevalence and relationship with conduct disorder. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1995;36:279–303. [PubMed: 7759591]
  • McCallon D. Diagnosing and treating ADHD in a men’s prison, The Science, Treatment and Prevention of Antisocial Behaviours: Application to the Criminal Justice System. Fishbein DH, editor. Kingston, New Jersey: Civic Research Institute; 2000. pp. 17.1–17.21.
  • Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Dickson N, et al. Childhood-onset versus adolescent-onset antisocial conduct in males: natural history from age 3 to 18. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:399–424.
  • Pliszka SR, Greenhill LL, Crismon ML, et al. The Texas Children’s Medication Algorithm Project: report of the Texas Consensus Conference Panel on medication treatment of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Parts I & II. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2000;39:908–927. [PubMed: 10892234]
  • Rosler M, Retz W, Retz-Junginger P, et al. Prevalence of attention deficit-/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and comorbid disorders in young male prison inmates. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 2004;254:365–371. [PubMed: 15538605]
  • Stephenson J, Goodman R. Association between behaviour at age 3 years and adult criminality. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:197–202. [PubMed: 11532795]
  • Swanson JA, Sargeant E, Taylor EJ, et al. Hyperkineses and ADHD. The Lancet. 1998;351:429–433.
  • Taylor E, Doepfner M, Sergeant J, et al. European clinical guidelines for hyperkinetic disorder – first upgrade. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;13(Suppl 1):1–30. [PubMed: 15322953]
  • Youth Justice Board . Asset-Young Offender Assessment Profile. 2006. Available at: http://www​.yjb.gov.uk​/en-gb/practitioners/Assessment/Asset​.htm.

1.3. CONCEPT OF HYPERKINETIC DISORDER AND ADHD AND ITS TREATMENT IMPLICATIONS

Authors

1.

Affiliations

1 Clinical Lecturer in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry

1.3.1. Introduction

This presentation will look at hyperkinetic disorder and ADHD. If you are looking at the symptom counts in the DSM, you need a greater number of symptoms than in the ICD-10 in order to make a diagnosis. However, an assumption that this means identifying fewer cases using DSM would be incorrect.

In hyperkinetic disorder, filters are applied, starting with the exclusion of anxiety and depression. You also need pervasiveness (symptoms across two settings) and impairment. (DSM focuses on impairment rather than symptoms across two settings.) Applying these filters mean that you will have a smaller number with hyperkinetic disorder as a diagnosis, as opposed to ADHD.

1.3.2. The MTA study

The MTA study (MTA Co-operative Group, 1999a & b; 2004a & b) can be used to show how figures can change by just using different criteria; for example the percentage of children diagnosed would vary whether parent, teacher or combined reports were used. Impairment and how you rate it and at what degree of impairment you say ‘it is important and needs treatment’ is relevant, because it is a question of how you set the threshold: that changes the numbers very dramatically. Differences in rates of diagnoses can therefore be explained by the way in which diagnostic criteria are applied.

1.3.3. Summary of the MTA study

This study was looked at to see whether the use of hyperkinetic disorder versus ADHD has an influence in terms of outcomes and treatment. (One factor to consider when looking at this study is that the intensity of the treatments used may not be transferable to clinical settings.)

The target population was children with a DSM-IV diagnosis of ADHD (combined type) plus a wide range of comorbid conditions and demographic characteristics. There were 579 individuals in the study group. The treatment strategies used in the randomly allocated groups were:

  • behavioural management (parent training, child-focused, school-based)
  • medication management (methylphenidate, if titration unsuccessful open titration of dextroamphetamine, pemoline, imipramine)
  • combined treatment
  • community care.

The combined treatment (medication plus behavioural intervention) was the most effective.

The next question to be asked was whether the MTA findings of combined ADHD could be generalised to hyperkinetic disorder. Starting with the initial 579 children with the diagnosis of ADHD (combined type), 147 were excluded for anxiety/depression. Of those remaining, once other filters were applied, 145 had a diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder.

Hyperkinetic disorder

One of the main findings was that if you had hyperkinetic disorder, then using stimulants would be a good option as you had a higher chance of responding to medication. Children with hyperkinetic disorder are prescribed stimulants; this will also be the case for children with oppositional defiant disorder/conduct disorder (behavioural therapy is not used).

Anxiety and depression

If you had anxiety and depression, it is the combined treatment that was important; not just the behavioural intervention, but behavioural plus medication would be better than medication alone.

Mild or ‘borderline’ ADHD

You could get the same response with either behavioural intervention or stimulant use. The treatment recommendation for ‘borderline’ ADHD is behavioural therapy, then stimulants. If this is not effective the diagnosis is reviewed.

Non-hyperkinetic disorder

The one thing that stood out clearly in the data set was that inattention being reported in schools actually seemed to be a predictor that medication helped the inattention in school. Here medication should be a reasonable choice.

Health economics

Medication usage was effective in terms of treatment and even the community care as usual was beneficial. If you look at intensive behavioural therapy versus community care, then if you had a diagnosis of hyperkinetic disorder, it was almost costing twice as much as the ADHD construct. If you had hyperkinetic disorder or hyperkinetic conduct disorder, the likelihood of the behavioural strategy alone working over the medication is going to be less cost effective.

Even the intensive behavioural strategy used in this study was never more effective than medication.

1.3.4. What are practical applications of the MTA study in clinical practice?

Possible models include:

  • telephone-based medication monitoring and stabilisation clinic – Cambridge International Primary Programme (CIPP)
  • 1-week MTA titration phase strategy
  • day-patient observation with differing doses of stimulants
  • intense monitoring offered only when routine treatment fails.

Do these strategies matter when we now have long-acting drugs?

NICE guidelines should also be trying to look at how clinicians can be helped to do better clinical monitoring and titrating, as opposed to just deciding whether someone needs to receive a drug or not.

1.4. PREDICTIVE VALIDITY OF BROAD VERSUS NARROW CLASSIFICATION OF HYPERACTIVITY

Authors

, MD, FRCP(C)1.

Affiliations

1 Department of Psychiatry, Neurosciences and Mental Health Research Institute, The Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Dr Russell Schachar, MD, FRCP(C)

Department of Psychiatry, Neurosciences and Mental Health Research Institute, The Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Address for correspondence: R. Schachar, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, M5G 1X8, Canada, ac.sdikkcis@rahcahcs.llessur

At one extreme of the debate about the validity of the diagnosis of childhood hyper-activity are those who assert that the diagnosis is invalid no matter what criteria are applied. More often, however, the question is framed around the appropriate breadth of the diagnosis. Some hold that only a narrowly defined syndrome such as hyperkinetic disorder as defined in ICD-10 has diagnostic validity and that a more broadly defined syndrome such as ADHD as defined in DSM-IV captures a group of children who either has no disorder whatsoever or a group who is similar to children with other and presumably more valid and clinically meaningful diagnoses such as conduct disorder. Figure 1 shows several hypothetic functions relating severity of the phenotype on the y axis and accumulating underlying risk on the x axis. A narrowly defined diagnostic entity is shown by the smaller oval, a broadly defined entity by the larger oval.

Figure 1. Models relation phenotype and risk.

Figure 1

Models relation phenotype and risk.

In Model A, risk accumulates slowly without behavioural, cognitive or other manifestations until some threshold is exceeded. Beyond that threshold, the disorder is manifest and further risk does not substantially alter the phenotype. This is essentially the pathogen-disease model of disorder.

Model B shows a variation of the first function. At some level of the trait, there is a substantial increase in the expression of the disorder just as in model A. But in model B, the narrowly defined entity misses many individuals with risk who are captured by the broader criteria. The big difference between broad and narrow entities is prevalence although the narrow entity could show more risks depending on the slope of the function relating risk to symptoms over the hypothetical diagnostic threshold. According to this model, both the broadly and the narrowly defined entities are different from unaffected individuals.

Model C shows a different function in which phenotypic expression increases linearly with increasing risk. There is no point at which there is a substantial and discontinuous increase in phenotypic expression with accumulating risk. Accordingly, a disorder defined narrowly by the presence of severe expression (the most symptomatic, the most impaired, those with the most evidence of some underlying pathogen or dysfunction) would differ in degree rather than in kind from a more broadly defined entity. Under these circumstances, there can be no easy solution to the classification problem. There will always be individuals who fall just below the boundary of the category and sub-threshold cases will differ only in degree from supra-threshold cases. Under these circumstances, factors other than validity of the defined entity will determine where the threshold is set. ICD-10 criteria are narrower than those for DSM in terms of pervasiveness, the range of symptoms required for criteria to be met (symptoms of inattention, hyperactivity and impulsiveness) and treatment of comorbidity.

We evaluated these models by assessing the predictive validity of hyperkinetic disorder and ADHD in a sample of approximately 1000 consecutive referrals to a specialty clinic for attention, learning and behaviour problems. First, we compared children who met criteria for hyperkinetic disorder, ADHD-combined subtype, ADHD-inattentive subtype, ADHD-hyperactive impulsive subtype and controls on a range of clinical and cognitive characteristics. Then we excluded cases with any comorbid condition (conduct disorder or oppositional disorder, generalised or separation anxiety disorder, reading disability) and compared hyperkinetic disorder, ADHD, and control groups once again. Only one in ten cases that met criteria for ADHD also fulfilled criteria for hyperkinetic disorder. The hyperkinetic disorder group was more severe in that they exhibited a greater number of parent- and teacher-rated symptoms followed by the ADHD-combined subtype, ADHD-hyperactive impulsive subtype and ADHD-inattentive subtype groups in descending order. Despite differences in symptoms severity and pervasiveness, hyperkinetic disorder, ADHD-combined subtype, ADHD-inattentive subtype and ADHD-hyperactive impulsive subtype differed little in teacher- and parent-rated impairment, exposure to psychosocial adversity (for example, low socioeconomic status, single parent-headed homes, and so on), recurrence risk for ADHD in first-degree family members, comorbidity (except for lower rate of conduct disorder in the ADHD-inattentive subtype group), intelligence, reading scores, and measures of working memory (digit span backward) and inhibitory control (stop signal reaction time in the stop task). All of these groups had more deviant or extreme scores in each of these characteristics than did controls. After excluding comorbidity, hyperkinetic disorder, ADHD and conduct disorder groups differed little in recurrence risk for ADHD in family members, exposure to psychosocial adversity, intelligence, digit span backwards, and reading performance; all three of these groups differed from controls. Hyperkinetic disorder was marked by more severe inhibitory control deficit than the ADHD, conduct disorder and control groups. The hyperkinetic disorder, conduct disorder and ADHD groups were more impaired according to parent and teacher ratings. In addition, parents rated the conduct disorder group as more impaired than the hyperkinetic disorder and ADHD groups whereas teachers rated the hyperkinetic disorder group as more impaired than the ADHD and conduct disorder groups.

In summary, these results support the predictive validity of both the narrowly (hyperkinetic disorder, ICD-10) and the broadly (ADHD, DSM-IV) defined entities and reject the hypothesis that either broadly or narrowly define hyperactivity or both are invalid clinical entities or nothing more than that which is predicted by their common comorbidities (Model A). There was only minimal evidence in these data for a quantitative increase in the severity of associated risks with increase in severity or pervasiveness (Model C). There was a trend for inhibitory control to be worse in hyperkinetic disorder compare with ADHD-combined subtype, ADHD-hyperactive impulsive subtype, and ADHD-inattentive subtype groups in that order. These results do not isolate a unique feature of childhood hyperactivity. In conclusion, the most clearly supported model is Model B which posits that both the broadly and the narrowly defined entities exceed the threshold for a valid diagnostic entity.

Finally, it should be recalled that in North America the small subgroup of narrowly defined hyperkinetic disorder cases would all meet criteria for ADHD; the predictive validity of the later group will be more marked than was found in this study where hyperkinetic disorder cases were separated from ADHD. By contrast, in the UK and other countries which follow ICD-10 diagnostic practice, at least nine of ten impaired children will not receive a diagnosis. More than half of these cases do not receive any other diagnosis and will therefore not receive a diagnosis commensurate with the seriousness of their disorder.

1.5. SOCIAL AND CULTURAL ISSUES IN ADHD DIAGNOSES AND PSYCHOSTIMULANT TREATMENT

Authors

1 and 2.

Affiliations

1 Wellcome Trust University Lecturer in Bioethics and Society, The London School of Economics and Political Science
2 Professor of Sociology, Convenor of Department of Sociology, The London School of Economics and Political Science

Dr Ilina Singh

Wellcome Trust University Lecturer in Bioethics and Society, The London School of Economics and Political Science

Professor Nikolas Rose

Professor of Sociology, Convenor of Department of Sociology, The London School of Economics and Political Science

Reliable diagnosis rates for ADHD are difficult to find in any national context. It is also difficult to know the true epidemiology of ADHD in any national context, and prevalence rates vary widely, from 0.5 to 26% in the UK; and from 2 to 18% in the US. There is, however, good systematic data on worldwide consumption of methylphenidate (and dexamfetamine), collected by the United Nations International Narcotics Control Board. There is also detailed data available from IMS Health.

Both these sources demonstrate an enormous variation in global consumption of methylphenidate. Average consumption rates increased dramatically between 1999 and 2003, averaging five- to seven-fold increases. There are many possible explanations for this variation, including (and not limited to) true epidemiological variation across countries in ADHD, the impact of national prescribing practices, medical training, parenting ideology, drug policies, health insurance, educational practices, teaching, and so forth. The bottom line is this: we don’t know why this variation exists.

The global variation in stimulant drug consumption does point to the fact that social and cultural factors are key to understanding patterns and trends in ADHD diagnoses and psychostimulant treatment. This does not mean that ADHD may not also have an organic aetiology. Socio-cultural analysis can make an important contribution to identifying and evaluating key environmental factors that shape ADHD diagnosis and stimulant drug treatment patterns.

It is unclear which level of socio-cultural analysis would be most useful. Potential analyses cover a wide range of targets: from a macro-level study of by-nation variation in methylphenidate consumption, to a micro-level study of the beliefs and practices of individual teachers and psychiatrists in local settings.

Evidence of socio-cultural factors in ADHD diagnosis and treatment can inform the guideline by providing understanding of the pathway to diagnosis of ADHD, and the key consequences of diagnosis of ADHD for the child and family. This is particularly important now that ADHD is no longer understood as a disorder of childhood. There is little or no long-term data on the ‘career’ of the ADHD patient. We need to understand more about this career in order to assess the risks and benefits of (1) a narrow versus wide diagnosis and (2) recommendations of long-term drug treatment.

We also need to avoid mistakenly attributing to the child consequences of social situations and cultural forces. This means we must have better (objective, sound and uniform) diagnoses for ADHD. However, even if this can be realised, in the absence of a biological marker for ADHD, there will always be an inherent dilemma about whether to cast the ADHD net widely or narrowly (by supporting a wide or a narrowly constructed diagnostic guideline). The costs and benefits of either approach must be very carefully weighed.

1.6. CATEGORICAL MODELS OF ATTENTION DEFICIT/HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER: A CONCEPTUAL AND EMPIRICAL ANALYSIS

Authors

1.

Affiliations

1 Wellcome Trust University Lecturer in Bioethics and Society, The London School of Economics and Political Science

In this presentation we explored the status of categorical models of ADHD as they underpin current diagnostic formulations in the DSM-IV and the ICD-10 and anchor debate about future revisions of these manuals. The presentation draws on the ideas published in a published journal article (Sonuga-Barke, 1998).61 The presentation had three major elements. Part one involved a discussion of the defining role of diagnostic systems in clinical and scientific practice related to ADHD. We reviewed the historical development of the role of diagnostic systems and their political and economic foundations. Part two was a review of three major themes relating to categorical models of childhood disorder.

First, we discussed the inevitability of categorisation in clinical practice given the imperative to identify those individuals in need of intervention, that is, clinicians are inevitably categorisers and so categorical diagnostic systems go with the grain of clinical practice. Furthermore, we highlight the social-psychological basis of categorical models of disorder by arguing that clinicians, like other humans, when faced with challenge of understanding complex human behaviour, tend to use heuristic devices that involve inferring traits on the basis of behavioural observations and drawing categorical boundaries even when these are not obviously present.

Second, we examined the relationship between clinical categorisation and science. Here we focused on the role that the values and assumptions inherent in categorical diagnostic systems and the way that influence scientific practice – the hypotheses that are tested and the methods that are used to test them. In assuming that disorders, such as ADHD, are discrete entities qualitatively different from the normal variation of behaviour we bias our search for categorical boundaries between normality and abnormality and over-interpret evidence in favour of the validity of conditions. However, there is a need for a bridge of common meaning between the ‘laboratory’ and the ‘clinic’ and categorical diagnostic models support this vital function.

Third, we considered the different ways that one could respond to this recognition of the role of assumptions in the scientific study of categorical models of disorder. After considering a number of options (including the rejection of diagnostic approaches on the grounds that they are social constructions) we argued that Meehl’s (1992) scientific realism whereby scientific assumptions are turned into specific testable hypotheses was the most valuable approach. The hypothesis that ADHD is a true category, or as Meehl (1992) calls it, a taxa, has not been tested sufficiently to date. However, genetics studies using DF analyses that look at the relationship between symptom severity and heritability do not support the taxa hypothesis of ADHD. More recent and more sophisticated studies using advanced taxonomic analyses also find no evidence for the existence of an ADHD taxa (for example, Haslam et al., 2006; Frazier et al., 2007). It appears that ADHD is better modelled as a continuous trait rather than a discrete category.

We concluded by highlighting the dilemma between this empirical reality (that ADHD is better regarded as the extreme of normal variation rather than a distinct category) and the practical necessity and psychological inevitability that clinicians will make categorical decisions. We concluded by highlighting concerns over the transparency, communicability and implementability of a dimensional system for the diagnosis of ADHD while accepting that it may be a better model for science. Adopting such a model in future diagnostic formulations may run the risk of dismantling the bridge of meaning between clinic and lab – paradoxically inhibiting the process of diagnostic refinement and so the relevance of scientific findings to clinical practice.

REFERENCES

  • Haslam N, Williams B, Prior M, et al. The latent structure of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a taxometric analysis. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;40:639–647. [PubMed: 16866759]
  • Frazier TW, Youngstrom EA, Naugle RI. The latent structure of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinic-referred sample. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:45–64. [PubMed: 17201529]
  • Meehl PE. Factors and taxa, traits and types, differences of degree and differences in kind. Journal of Personality. 1992;60:117–174.
  • Sonuga-Barke EJS. Categorical model in child psychopathology: a conceptual and empirical analysis. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:15–133. [PubMed: 9534089]

1.7. ARGUMENTS AGAINST THE USE OF THE CONCEPT IN CLINICAL PRACTICE: INCLUDING WHETHER IT SHOULD BE USED NEVER OR SPARINGLY

Authors

1.

Affiliations

1 Consultant Child Psychiatrist, Lincolnshire Partnership NHS Trust

The main problem with current theory and practice in ADHD is the prevalence of the underlying assumption that ADHD is a genetic neurodevelopmental disorder and that clinicians have valid and reliable ways of identifying what behaviours are the result of such neurodevelopmental disabilities in any individual child. This narrow biomedical construction causes a polarisation of views and attitudes with proponents of this view claiming ‘there is no such disagreement [about ADHD being a valid disorder]—at least no more so than there is over whether smoking causes cancer, or whether a virus causes HIV/AIDS’ (Barkley, 2002);62 while opponents claim, ‘It’s as simple as this: if no physical examination, lab test, X-ray, scan or biopsy shows an abnormality in your children, your child is normal’ (Baughman,1998).

Current evidence does not support a simplistic view of ADHD type behaviours. Genetic studies have relied on poor standards of evidence (such as the disputed ‘equal environment’ assumption), and have failed to replicate genetic associations consistently, thus the null hypothesis stands – no genes exist for ADHD. Similarly, neuro-imaging studies suffer from serious methodological failings and interpretive inadequacies; thus there are currently no neurological markers for ADHD (nor are there likely to be). Conceptual problems are endemic in ADHD; these include: high comorbidity, cross-cultural variations among raters and the rated, the behaviours are qualitatively common behaviours leading to large variations in prevalence, the gender distribution and the circularity of construct (the behaviours define the disorder, the disorder defines the behaviours).

The most important implication of the dominance of biological theory in ADHD is that it has led to a rapid rise in the use of biological remedies as the first-line and often only treatment for those diagnosed with ADHD. This is problematic. Although stimulants have proven efficacy (up to 4 weeks), the long-term outcome literature available does not support stimulants being effective in the long term (an important finding given that many end up on stimulants for many years). Current treatment protocols have come to rely too heavily on the MTA study (1999). However this had major methodological and interpretive flaws, with the 24-month follow-up study (MTA, 2004) having less positive findings for medication, with children on medication experiencing significant side effects. Indeed William Pelham, who was on the board of the MTA studies, recently concluded: ‘No drug company in its literature mentions the fact that 40 years of research says there is no long-term benefit of medications. That is something parents need to know.’ (Quoted in Hearn, 2004). The literature on medication has exaggerated stimulants’ effectiveness and minimised its risks (which include serious risks such as cardiac disease, psychosis and sudden death).

However, we still have the reality that many children and many families are struggling to understand and deal with a range of behavioural and educational problems that we currently call ADHD. Some appear to benefit from diagnosis and prescription of medication, but we must balance this with our social responsibility for public health.

Alternative and useful ways forward can be found through incorporating discourses and research from related fields such as philosophy and transcultural/anthropological psychiatry which can provide both theory and practice with conceptual and practical tools to engage with questions of values, ethics, diversity and the changing nature of the challenges and circumstances that children and families live in.

The implications of this line of thinking are many. For the purposes of guidelines in diagnosing ADHD this means that ADHD should not be viewed as neurodevelopmental; diagnosis should come under the remit of mental health not paediatrics; the diagnosis should be reserved for more serious cases that are not responding to a variety of currently available clinical approaches, and when a diagnosis is made this should not lead to a long-term prescription.

REFERENCES

  • Baughman FA. The totality of the ADD/ADHD fraud. National Institutes of Health Consensus Conference on ADHD. 1998. Video available at http://www​.adhdfraud.com.
  • Barkley R. International Consensus Statement on ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review. 2002;5:89–111. [PubMed: 12093014]
  • Hearn K. Here kiddie, kiddie. 2004. [accessed June 2005]. Available at http://alternet​.org/drugreporter/20594/
  • MTA Co-operative Group. A 14 month randomized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:1073–1086. [PubMed: 10591283]
  • MTA Co-operative Group. National Institute of Mental Health Multimodal Treatment Study of ADHD follow-up: 24-month outcomes of treatment strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2004;113:754–761. [PubMed: 15060224]

PART 2. DRAFT DIAGNOSIS CHAPTER (PART 1) SENT TO PEER REVIEWERS63

Part I - Validity of the ADHD diagnosis

1.1. INTRODUCTION

This guideline is applicable to people above the age of three and of all levels of intellectual ability who show symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity or inattention to a degree that impairs their academic progress, mental development, personal relationships, or physical or mental health. This includes people with ADHD whether or not they have other comorbid mental disorders or whether the ADHD symptoms result from genetic, physical environmental or social environmental causes. This chapter sets out to look at the issues of diagnostic categorisation and assessment that should trigger the use of this guideline. First, we address the validity of DSM-IV ADHD and ICD-10 hyperkinetic disorder as diagnostic categories. Second, a guidance for clinical practice is provided.

1.2. THE VALIDITY OF ADHD AS A DIAGNOSTIC CATEGORY

The Guideline Development Group (GDG) acknowledged at the outset that the use of the diagnosis of ADHD has been the subject of considerable controversy and debate and that the diagnosis itself has varied across time and place as diagnostic systems have evolved (Rhodes et al., 2006). Points of controversy identified by the GDG included the reasons for the wide variation in prevalence rates reported for ADHD and the nature of the aetiological risk factors for ADHD.

The GDG wished to evaluate evidence for the validity of the diagnostic category of ADHD and formulate a position statement. It was recognised that defining psychiatric disorders is a difficult process due to the overlapping nature of behavioural and psychiatric syndromes, the complexity of the aetiological processes and the lack of a ‘gold standard’ such as a biological test – in this regard ADHD is no different from other common psychiatric disorders. Furthermore, in keeping with other common behavioural disorders there is no clear distinction between the clinical condition and the normal variation in the general population (see Section A3 [1.4.3]). This is comparable to normal variation for medical traits such as hypertension and type II diabetes, as well as psychological problems such as anxiety. Controversial issues surround changing thresholds applied to the definition of illness as new knowledge and treatments are developed (Kessler et al., 2003 and the extent to which functioning within the ‘normal cultural environment’ should determine clinical thresholds (Sonuga-Barke, 1998; Rosenman, 2006). As a result of considering these issues, a central question for this chapter is to delineate the level of ADHD symptoms and associated impairments required to trigger the use of this guideline.

It was recognised from the start that undertaking a systematic review of diagnostic categories is not a straightforward exercise for behavioural and psychiatric disorders because in most cases definitive diagnostic tests for the presence or absence of disorder do not exist. The relative lack of a validated reference standard (indicated by SIGN diagnostic study quality assessment, see Appendix A64) means that the question of validity for the diagnosis of ADHD needs to draw on evidence from a wide range of sources. There is also potential for ascertainment bias particularly in clinic-referred populations and considerable variation by clinical and demographic subgroups, disease prevalence and severity, and use of different behavioural and symptom measures (Whiting et al., 2004). The GDG wish to emphasise that psychiatric nosology is a dynamic and developing field and changes are to be expected over time as more data are accrued.

To ensure that a transparent, structured approach was taken, the GDG agreed to use one similar to the Washington University Diagnostic Criteria (Feighner et al., 1972). This approach involves setting out criteria for validating a particular disorder and seeing how far a particular set of phenomena is consistent with those criteria. Using these criteria as a framework, this chapter sets out to answer the following questions:

  1. To what extent do the phenomena of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention, which define the current DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria for ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder, cluster together in the general population and into a particular disorder that can be distinguished from other disorders and from normal variation?
  2. Is the cluster of symptoms that defines ADHD associated with significant clinical and psychosocial impairments?
  3. Is there evidence for a characteristic pattern of developmental changes, or outcomes associated with the symptoms, that define ADHD?
  4. Is there consistent evidence of genetic, environmental or neurobiological risk factors associated with ADHD?

These questions were taken to relate to both DSM-IV ADHD and ICD-10 hyper-kinetic disorder criteria. Hyperkinetic disorder is a more restricted definition of ADHD that forms a subset of the DSM-IV combined subtype of ADHD. The term ‘hyperactivity’ has been used in some studies to mean the cluster of hyperactive, impulsive and inattentive symptoms. In this guideline the term ‘hyperactivity’ is restricted to mean the combination of symptoms that defines overactive behaviour and the term ‘ADHD symptoms’ is used to refer to the combination of hyperactive, impulsive and inattentive symptoms.

1.3. METHODOLOGY

A literature search was conducted for existing systematic reviews and meta-analyses on CINAHL, EMBASE, MEDLINE, PsycINFO. The initial search found 5,516 reviews of which nine were relevant to the questions about ADHD and application of the Washington University Diagnostic Criteria. Where insufficient evidence was found from previous systematic reviews, a search for primary studies was carried out (see Appendix B). We selected reviews for inclusion in this chapter if they met the SIGN quality assessment criteria for systematic reviews and cohort studies. For diagnostic and factor analytic studies we established a set of criteria approved by NICE (Appendix C).

In addition to the review of the literature, a consensus conference was held to bring together experts in the field who held a range of views and could address the concept of ADHD from different perspectives. This provided an opportunity to debate the key issues surrounding the use of this diagnostic category and thereby to assist the GDG with the task of deciding what should trigger the use of the guideline and for whom the guideline is intended. A summary of the consensus conference is provided in an Appendix to this chapter (Appendix D).

1.4. REVIEWING THE VALIDITY OF THE DIAGNOSIS: SUMMARY OF THE EVIDENCE

A: To what extent do the phenomena of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention, which define the current DSM-IV and ICD-10 criteria for ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder, cluster together in the general population and into a particular disorder that can be distinguished from other disorders and from normal variation?

The evidence addressing this issue is divided into three main questions:

A1.

Do the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity cluster together?

A2.

Are ADHD symptoms distinguishable from other conditions?

A3.

Are the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity distinguishable from the normal spectrum?

1.4.1. (A1) Do the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity cluster together?

No evidence was found from the systematic search of reviews that was of direct relevance to this question. This is because, despite a large primary literature, few systematic reviews in this area have been undertaken. Therefore a systematic search of factor-analytic studies was carried out. Additional factor-analytic and cross-sectional studies were identified by the GDG (Appendix E). None of these studies met the SIGN inclusion criteria that requires an appropriate reference standard for diagnostic measures, but did meet the extension to the SIGN criteria approved for this review: the aim of the question was to evaluate whether the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity cluster together in the population, rather than to assess the accuracy of diagnostic tests.

The inclusion criteria for factor-analytic studies were defined as follows: (i) that the study addresses an appropriate and clearly focused question and (ii) that the sample population being studied was selected either as a consecutive series or randomly, from a clearly defined study population.

Evidence

Many factor analyses indicate a two-factor model; ‘hyperactivity-impulsivity’ and ‘inattention’. This has been replicated in population-based studies (Lahey et al., 1994; Leviton et al., 1993; Wolraich et al., 1996) and clinical samples (Bauermeister et al., 1992; Lahey et al., 1988; Pelham et al., 1992). Single factor ‘hyperactivity-impulsivity’ is also supported by Dreger and colleagues’ (1964) early study where the factor ‘hyperactivity’ was defined as ‘impulsive, excitable hyperactivity’. More recent factor-analytic studies based on DSM-IV criteria support previous findings that the symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity are distinct symptom domains in children (Molina et al., 2001; Amador-Campos et al., 2005; Zuddas et al., 2006) and adolescents (Hudziak et al., 1998).

Looking specifically at children identified as having a behavioural problem Conners (1969) found ‘hyperactivity’ and ‘inattention’ as separate and distinct factors. The factor structure of adolescent self-report behavioural data was investigated by Conners and colleagues (1997) and found six factors including ‘hyperactivity’ and ‘cognitive problems’. The ‘hyperactivity’ factor included characteristics such as being unable to sit still for very long, squirming and fidgeting and feeling restless inside when sitting still. The ‘cognitive problems’ factor consisted of having trouble keeping focused attention, having problems organising tasks and forgetting things that were learnt. Similar results were found in Conners and colleagues’ (1998) further study were attentional problems that overlap with the DSM-IV criteria for inattentive subtype of ADHD, with a similar overlap between the factor items and DMS-IV criteria for hyperactivity-impulsivity [sic].

Some studies have identified three factors; ‘hyperactivity’ and ‘impulsivity’ as two distinct factors in addition to ‘inattention’ in both the general population (Gomez et al., 1999; Glutting et al., 2005) and clinical populations (Pillow et al., 1998). However, Gomez and colleagues (1999) showed that the model fit for the three-factor solution was only marginally better than the two-factor model. In the study of Pillow and colleagues (1998) of boys with ADHD, the impulsive and hyperactive symptoms formed a single factor when oppositional-defiant and conduct disorder items were also included in the factor analysis.

Werry and colleagues (1975), however, found that hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention formed a single factor using both population control and ‘hyperactive’ samples.

Using a latent class analysis (LCA) that identifies clusters of symptoms that group together, Hudziak and colleagues (1998) found that hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention could exist as a ‘combined’ type latent class as well as separate hyperactive-impulsive and inattention latent classes. The latent classes map closely to the DSM-IV criteria, with DSM-IV combined type falling entirely within the severe combined type latent class. Individuals with the DSM-IV inattentive subtype fell either within the severe inattentive or the severe combined latent classes.

The clustering of hyperactivity, impulsivity and inattention appear to be stable across a number of countries. Ho and colleagues (1996) found separate robust dimensions for ‘hyperactivity’ (the combination of inattention and hyperactive-impulsive behaviour), ‘antisocial’ and ‘neurotic’ behaviour in a sample of 3,069 Chinese schoolboys. Correlations among different dimensions were similar to those reported in European and US samples. Taylor and Sandberg (1984) compared data from 437 English schoolchildren with published data from the US and New Zealand. They identified a factor of hyperactivity-inattention that was distinct from conduct disorder. The comparisons supported the view that English schoolchildren were similar to their contemporaries in the US and New Zealand with differences in prevalence rates between different countries accounted for by discrepancies in diagnostic practice.

In adult population samples a two-factor model has been identified (DuPaul et al., 2001; Smith & Johnson, 2000) as well as a three-factor model (Kooij et al., 2005). Glutting and colleagues (2005) assessed university students aged 17 to 22 using parent-rated information in addition to self-rated data. They reported slightly contrasting findings within each set of data; exploratory and confirmatory analysis showed that DSM-IV ADHD symptoms generated a three-factor model in the self-report data and a two-factor model in the parent-informant data.

Although most studies show separate factors for ‘inattention’ and ‘hyperactivity-impulsivity’, these are highly correlated in children (Gomez et al., 1999) and adult samples (Kooij et al., 2005).

There may be age-dependent changes in the factor structure. Bauermeister and colleagues (1992) found that there was a single ‘attention/impulsivity-hyperactivity’ factor in pre-school children, and separation into two factors in school-age children. Nearly all the studies of school-age children reported two factors. In contrast, the study from Glutting and colleagues (2005) using college students aged 17 to 22 found three factors, with the separation of hyperactive and impulsive symptoms. Similarly Kooij and colleagues (2005) using adult samples identified three separate factors.

Summary

Factor-analytic studies indicate that ADHD symptoms cluster together in general population samples. The number of factors varies between studies, with most finding two correlated factors for hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention; others find that hyperactivity and impulsivity can be distinguished and a few find one combined factor of all three domains. These findings have been observed in both population and clinical samples and in a number of different cultural settings. LCA in population samples detects clustering of symptoms into groups that are similar but not identical to DSM-IV criteria for ADHD.

1.4.2. (A2) Are ADHD symptoms distinguishable from other conditions?

No systematic reviews were identified in the literature that addressed this question. The GDG considered that the most important and controversial distinction to be made was between ADHD and oppositional-defiant and conduct disorders. These are also the most commonly reported comorbid problems in children and adolescents diagnosed with ADHD and define a set of behaviours that might be difficult to distinguish from ADHD. It was therefore decided to restrict a formal literature search to identify studies that indicate whether a distinction can be made between ADHD, oppositional-defiant and conduct problems. Additional references were identified by the GDG members (see Appendix F).

Evidence
1.4.2.1. ADHD and oppositional-defiant and conduct problems

Most of the studies using factor-analytic approaches for the analysis of ADHD symptoms report separate factors for hyperactivity-impulsivity, inattention and oppositional-defiant or conduct problems. These include most of the studies reviewed in the previous section on factor structure of ADHD symptoms (for example, Bauermeister et al., 1992; Conners, 1969; Conners, 1997a & 1997b; Ho et al., 1996; Pelham et al., 1992; Taylor & Sandberg, 1984; Werry et al., 1975; Wolraich et al., 1996). These studies are highly consistent in being able to separate oppositional-defiant and conduct problems from hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention. Although the symptoms fall into separate dimensions there are significant correlations between the behavioural factors.

Frouke and colleagues (2005) conducted a diagnostic study of 2,230 Dutch pre-adolescents from the general population. LCA revealed that ADHD symptoms clustered together with symptoms of oppositional-defiant disorder and conduct disorder. A further study from the Netherlands of disruptive behaviour in 636 7-year-old children (Pol et al., 2003) came to similar conclusions. LCA using the same data identified three main classes of children with: (i) high levels of ODD (oppositional defiant disorder) and ADHD, (ii) intermediate levels of ODD and ADHD with low levels of CP (conduct problems) and (iii) low levels of all disruptive problems. No classes were identified with only ADHD, ODD or CP.

King and colleagues (2005a) identified seven distinct groups using a cluster analysis that identified discrete groups: ADHD with inattention (ADHD-I), ADHD with hyperactivity-impulsivity (ADHD-H/I), ADHD with both hyperactivity/impulsivity and inattention (ADHD-C), ADHD-C with oppositional defiant disorder, and ADHD-I with oppositional defiant disorder. For both the inattentive symptoms and combined inattentive/hyperactive-impulsive symptoms they found clustering either with or without symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder.

Latent dimension modelling by Ferguson and colleagues (1991) looking at children with ADHD and conduct disorder (CD) suggested that these could be seen as independent dimensions, although they are highly inter-correlated. However the two often occur independently of each other and only partially share aetiological factors.

ADHD can be a precursor of other problems. When ADHD and disruptive behavioural problems coexist the history usually suggests that symptoms of ADHD appear first before the development of disruptive behavioural problems. A follow-up of a community sample of children with ADHD symptoms but no oppositional behaviour between the ages of 7 and 17 revealed that children with ADHD symptoms could develop oppositional behaviour at a later stage, but that the reverse pathway from oppositional behaviour to ADHD was uncommon (Taylor et al., 1996).

Population twin studies find that symptoms of ADHD are distinct from but share overlapping familial and genetic influences with conduct problems (Thapar et al., 2001; Silberg et al., 1996; Nadder et al., 2002). Multivariate twin modelling suggests that while the genetic influences on conduct disorder are largely shared with those that influence ADHD, there are in addition important environmental factors that influence the risk for conduct problems but not ADHD (Thapar et al., 2001). Nadder and colleagues (2002) conclude that the co-variation of ADHD and ODD/CD is the result of shared genetic influences with little influence from environmental factors. However there are substantial environmental influences on ODD/CD, especially when they are not accompanied by ADHD (Silberg et al., 1996; Eaves et al., 1997). The heritability of ADHD symptoms is also higher than that for ODD/CD symptoms in these studies.

1.4.2.2. ADHD and other co-occurring conditions

Population twin studies find that symptoms of ADHD are distinct from but share overlapping familial and genetic influences with other neurodevelopmental problems including reading difficulties (Gilger et al., 1992; Willcutt et al., 2000; Willcutt et al., 2007), impaired general cognitive ability (Kuntsi et al., 2004) and developmental coordination disorder (Martin et al., 2006).

ADHD is reported to co-occur with personality disorder in young offenders (Young et al., 2003). A prison survey found that 45% of incarcerated young adults had a previous history and persistence of ADHD symptoms (Rosler et al., 2004). The distinction between ADHD and personality disorder in adults raises important nosological questions and remains poorly investigated.

Dysthymia, depression and anxiety symptoms and disorders are frequently associated with ADHD in adults. In the US National Comorbidity Survey, adults with ADHD had increased rates of mood disorders, anxiety disorders, substance misuse disorders and impulse control disorders (Kessler et al., 2006).

Summary

In the majority of factor-analytic studies ADHD symptoms are found to represent separate but correlated factors from oppositional behaviour and conduct problems. When symptom clusters are considered, ADHD symptoms are often found to group together with oppositional behaviour. Longitudinal studies suggest that ADHD represents a separate condition that is a risk factor for the development of oppositional and conduct problems. Twin studies suggest overlapping genetic influences on ADHD and conduct problems but the genetic influences estimated by twin studies are greater for ADHD than ODD/CD and there are environmental influences on ODD/CD that do not act on ADHD. The correlation between ADHD and several neurodevelopmental traits (cognitive ability, reading ability, developmental coordination and pervasive developmental disorders) is due largely to the effects of shared genetic influences. In adults, co-occurring symptoms, syndromes and disorders are frequently found to exist alongside the core ADHD syndrome, but their distinction from ADHD and the reasons for high rates of co-occurrence are not well addressed in the current literature.

1.4.3. (A3) Are the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity distinguishable from the normal spectrum?

No systematic reviews were identified that were of direct relevance to this question. The previous search for primary studies revealed two factor-analytic studies relevant to this question. Also, the GDG members identified further factor-analytic and genetic studies (see Appendix G).

Evidence

Many studies have found a strong correspondence between quantitative measures of ADHD and the categorical diagnosis (Biederman et al., 1993; Bird et al., 1987; Biederman et al., 1996; Boyle et al., 1997; Chen et al., 1994; Edelbrock et al., 1986). These studies show that children with ADHD appear to be at one extreme of a quantitative dimension and this on this [sic] quantitative dimension there is no obvious bi-modality that separates children with ADHD from non-ADHD children.

Twin studies using individual differences approaches (reviewed in Thapar et al., 1999; Faraone et al., 2005) and De Fries-Fulker extremes analysis (Gjone et al., 1996; Levy et al., 1997; Willcutt et al., 2000; Price et al., 2001) estimate similar heritability for ADHD symptoms from general population twin samples. These studies indicate that the genetic influences on ADHD are distributed throughout the population; there is no obvious threshold or cut-off between ADHD and the continuous distribution of symptoms in the population.

ADHD can be divided into multiple latent class groups distinguished on the basis of three symptom groupings: attention, hyperactivity-impulsivity and the combination of these two symptom domains. In addition, the symptom groups are separated on the basis of low, medium and high levels into distinct severity groups. Twin data from female adolescents in Missouri and children in Australia both found that a similar pattern of familial segregation for the latent classes suggesting that familial influences can distinguish between ADHD and the normal range of behaviour (Rasmussen et al., 2004). These data provide some evidence for the distinction of ADHD into inattentive, combined and hyperactive-impulsive subtypes and suggest that ADHD might be distinguishable from the normal range on the basis of familial risks to siblings.

Summary

Most analytic approaches are unable to make a clear distinction between the diagnosis of ADHD and the continuous distribution of ADHD symptoms in the general population. Twin studies suggest that familial and genetic influences on groups with extremely high ADHD symptom scores are the same as those that influence ADHD symptom levels throughout the general population. LCA can however be used to distinguish groups with high, moderate and low ADHD symptom levels and suggests that these groups might be distinguished on the basis of familial risk factors. The current literature does not address the difference in interpretation of the latent class and quantitative approaches. The GDG concluded that on the basis of current evidence, ADHD was similar to other common medical and psychiatric conditions that represent the extreme of dimensional traits such as hypertension, obesity, anxiety and depression.

1.4.4. B: Is the cluster of symptoms that defines ADHD associated with significant clinical and psychosocial impairments?

There were no systematic reviews that addressed this question. A search for cohort studies was carried out and additional primary studies were identified by the GDG members (see Appendix H).

Evidence
1.4.4.1. Academic difficulties

Follow-up studies of people diagnosed with ADHD in childhood have consistently indicated impairment in their academic functioning. Children and adolescents with ADHD have been shown to have greater impaired attention, less impulse control, greater off-task, restless and vocal behaviour (Fischer et al., 1990), poor reading skills (McGee et al., 1992) and speech and language problems (Hinshaw, 2002) when compared with healthy controls. These impairments often lead to grade retention (Hinshaw, 2002) and to a lower probability of completing schooling when compared with children who do not have ADHD (Mannuzza et al., 1993), suggesting potential long-term ramifications for vocational, social and psychological functioning into adulthood (Biederman et al., 1996; Young et al., 2005a & 2005b; Wilson & Marcotte, 1996).

An important question about educational impairment of children with ADHD is whether this is determined primarily by the presence of high levels of ADHD symptoms or the association with co-occurring conditions such as conduct disorder. Wilson and Marcotte (1996) found that the presence of ADHD in adolescents increased the risk for lower academic performance and poorer social, emotional and adaptive functioning, but that the additional presence of conduct disorder further increased the risk for maladaptive outcomes. In another study the association of conduct disorder with academic underachievement was found to be due to its comorbidity with ADHD (Frick et al., 1991).

1.4.4.2. Family difficulties

Impaired family relationships have been reported in families of children with ADHD. Follow-up studies indicate that mothers of children and adolescents with ADHD have more difficulty in child behaviour management practices and coping with their child’s behaviour (August et al., 1998), and display higher rates of conflict behaviours, such as negative comments, social irritability, hostility and maladaptive levels of communication and involvement (August et al., 1998; Fletcher et al., 1996).

Family impairment also permeates the parent’s lives. Parents of children with ADHD report having less time to meet their own needs, fewer close friendships, greater peer rejection and less time for family activities, which might lead to less family cohesion and a significant effect on the parent’s emotional health (Bagwell et al., 2001).

1.4.4.3. Social difficulties

Girls with ADHD tend to have fewer friends (Blachman & Hinshaw, 2002) and greater problems with peers and the opposite sex (Young et al., 2005a & 2005b).

Hyperactive children with or without conduct problems have higher rates of problems with peers and higher rates of social problems because of lack of constructive social activities (Taylor et al., 1996). In a study by Ernhardt and Hinshaw (1994) it was reported that a diagnosis of ADHD significantly predicted peer rejection; however aggressive and non-compliant disruptive behaviours were important and accounted for 32% of the variance in peer rejection.

1.4.4.4. Antisocial behaviour

Antisocial behaviour is more prevalent in children and adolescents with ADHD than non-ADHD groups. Some studies show increased rates of antisocial acts (for example, drug misuse) in comparison to children who do not have ADHD (Barkley et al., 2004; Mannuzza et al., 1998).

Follow-up studies have also shown that people with high levels of ADHD symptoms had significantly higher juvenile and adult arrest rates (Satterfield & Schell, 1997). Young adults with a diagnosis of ‘hyperactivity’ in childhood were more likely to have a diagnosis of antisocial disorder than healthy controls (32% versus 8%) and drug misuse (10% versus 1%) at follow-up (Mannuzza et al., 1991).

ADHD is also a risk factor for psychiatric problems including persistent hyperactivity, violence, antisocial behaviours (Biederman et al., 1996; Taylor et al., 1996), (Taylor et al., 1996) and antisocial personality disorder (Mannuzza et al., 1998).

In a prospective follow-up of 103 males diagnosed with ADHD, the presence of an antisocial or conduct disorder almost completely accounted for the increased risk for criminal activities. Mannuzza and colleagues (2002) reported that antisocial disorder was more prevalent in children with pervasive and school-only ADHD. However, Lee and Hinshaw (2004) reported that the predictive power of ADHD status to adolescent delinquency diminishes when key indices of childhood externalising behaviour related to ADHD are taken into account.

Boys with ADHD and high defiance ratings show significantly higher felony rates than healthy controls (Satterfield et al., 1994). However, ADHD diagnosed in childhood increases the risk of later antisocial behaviour, even in the absence of comorbid ODD or CD (Mannuzza et al., 2004).

1.4.4.5. Other problems

A 10-year prospective study of young people with ADHD found that the lifetime prevalence for all categories of psychopathology were significantly greater in young adults with ADHD compared with controls. This included markedly elevated rates of antisocial, addictive, mood and anxiety disorders (Biederman et al., 2006b).

Both cross-sectional epidemiological studies and follow-up studies of children with ADHD show increased rates of unemployment compared with controls (Biederman et al., 2006b; Kessler et al., 2006; Barkley et al., 2006). Adults with ADHD were found to have significantly lower educational performance and attainment, with 32% failing to complete high school; they had been fired from more jobs and were rated by employers as showing a lower job performance (Barkley et al., 2006). The survey from Biederman and colleagues (2006b) showed that 33.9% of people with ADHD were employed full time versus 59% of controls.

Summary

ADHD symptoms are associated with a range of impairments in social, academic, family, mental health and employment outcomes. Longitudinal studies indicate that ADHD symptoms specifically are associated with both current and future impairments; additional impairments also result from the presence of co-occurring conditions, in particular conduct problems. Adults with ADHD are found to have lower paid jobs and lower socioeconomic status. Impairment is an essential factor to be considered in the diagnosis of ADHD. While it is clear that the presence of high levels of ADHD symptoms is associated with impairment in multiple domains, it is not possible to delineate clearly a specific number of ADHD symptoms at which impairment arises.

1.4.5. C: Is there evidence for a characteristic pattern of developmental changes, or outcomes associated with the symptoms, that define ADHD?

The search for systematic reviews and meta-analyses identified one review that was of relevance to this question. Additional reviews and primary studies were identified by the GDG members (see Appendix I).

Evidence

There is evidence for continuity of ADHD symptoms over the lifespan. Faraone and colleagues (2006) analysed data from 32 follow-up studies of children with ADHD into adulthood. Where full criteria for ADHD were used approximately 15% of children were still diagnosed with ADHD at age 25. In addition, the meta-analysis found that approximately 65% of children by age 25 fulfilled the DSM-IV definition of ADHD ‘in partial remission’, indicating persistence of some symptoms of ADHD associated with continued clinically meaningful impairments.

Relative to controls, levels of overactivity and inattention are developmentally stable (Taylor et al., 1996). Longitudinal studies of children with ADHD show similar rates of ADHD in adolescence (Biederman et al., 1996; Faraone et al., 2002; Molina & Pelham, 2003).

Population twin studies have also addressed the stability of ADHD symptoms throughout childhood and adolescence. Rietveld and colleagues (2004) reported that parent ratings of attentional problems were moderately stable from age three to seven, and greater stability from age seven to ten. They further showed that such stability appeared to be mediated largely by overlapping genetic influences such that most, but not all, genetic influences at one age influenced ADHD at another age. Price and colleagues (2005) reported similar findings with correlations around 0.5 between ADHD symptoms at ages 2, 3 and 4. This stability was estimated to be mediated 91% by genetic influences. Kuntsi and colleagues (2004) extended these data to age 8, and found similar moderate stability between the data for age 2, 3 and 4 and the data for age 8. Larsson and colleagues (2004) completed a similar longitudinal twin study of 8 to 13 year olds and found fairly high stability between the two ages; they further concluded that this stability was due to shared genetic effects. Change in symptoms between childhood and adolescence was thought to be due to new genetic and environmental effects that become important in adolescence.

In adolescence and adult life, symptoms of ADHD begin to associate with other diagnoses that are seldom made in childhood. Adolescent substance misuse, in particular, seems to be more common in people with the diagnosis of ADHD (Wilens et al., 2003), though it is not yet clear whether it is the ADHD per se that generates the risk or the co-existent presence of antisocial activities and peer groups. The mechanisms involved can include one or more of the following: first, that individuals with ADHD may seek out highly stimulating or risky activities; second, that individuals with ADHD are exposed to higher levels of psychosocial risks for development of substance use disorders, resulting from educational and social impairments, social exclusion and antisocial behaviour associated with ADHD; third, that various substances, including cannabis, alcohol and stimulants can attenuate ADHD symptoms and are therefore sometimes used as a form of self-treatment.

Summary

There is evidence for the persistence of ADHD symptoms from early childhood through to adulthood. Longitudinal studies confirm that ADHD persists into adulthood but developmentally appropriate criteria have yet to be developed for ADHD in adults. Using child criteria, approximately 15% of children with ADHD retain the diagnosis by age 25 but a much larger proportion (65%) show persistence of symptoms with associated impairments. The profile of symptoms may alter with a relative persistence of inattentive symptoms compared with hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, however the evidence base for this conclusion is poor, using developmentally appropriate measures of hyperactivity-impulsivity in adults. There was no evidence to warrant a different diagnostic concept in childhood and in adulthood. Familial and genetic influences in ADHD symptoms appear to be stable through childhood and early adolescence, but there is a lack of data on the continuity of aetiological factors into adulthood.

1.4.6. D: Is there consistent evidence of genetic, environmental or neurobiological risk factors associated with ADHD?

The literature search identified seven systematic reviews and meta-analyses. GDG members identified additional reviews and primary studies (see Appendix J).

Evidence

Dickstein and colleagues (2006) completed a systematic meta-analysis of 16 neuroimaging studies that compared patterns of neural activity in children and adults with ADHD and healthy controls. Their results indicated a consistent pattern of reduced frontal activity (hypoactivity) in people with ADHD.

Willcutt and colleagues (2005) reviewed 83 studies that had administered executive functioning measures and found significant differences between ADHD and non-ADHD groups where the former showed executive function deficits. The size of the difference between children with ADHD and unaffected controls while significant was moderate rather than large.

Differences in executive functioning between ADHD and non-ADHD groups have also been reported in adults (Hervey et al., 2004; Boonstra et al., 2005; Schoechlin & Engel, 2005) results of studies of ADHD in adults suggest a wide variety of general and specific performance on cognitive-experimental tasks that are similar to those seen in children. The review from Hervey and colleagues (2004) did not point to a domain-specific neuropsychological deficit, but rather multiple domains revealed some degree of impairment on at least a subset of the tests considered within each domain. The interpretation of these studies remains controversial but most authorities agree that both executive and non-executive processes are disrupted in people with ADHD. Recently it has emerged that the strongest and most consistent association with ADHD is for intra-individual variability (Klein et al., 2006).

A systematic meta-analysis of molecular genetic association for associated markers in or near to the dopamine D4 (DRD4), dopamine D5 (DRD5) and dopamine transporter (DAT1) genes, found strong evidence for the association of DRD4 and DRD5 but not DAT1 (Li et al., 2006).

A systematic review of 20 population twin studies found an average heritability estimate of 76%. In most cases, heritability in these studies is estimated from the difference in the correlations for ADHD symptoms between identical and non-identical twin pairs, as reported by parents and teachers: with the correlation for identical twin pairs in the region of 60–90% and for non-identical twin pairs being half or less than half of this figure in most studies (Faraone, 2005). Under the equal environment assumption for the two types of twin pairs heritability can be estimated as twice the difference in the two sets of correlations. Although some people question the assumption of ‘equal environment’ for identical and non-identical twins, this does not impact on the question of validity since the high twin correlations observed in these studies indicates that ADHD symptoms are highly familial. The equal environment assumption impacts on estimates of the proportion of the familial risk that is due to genes or equal environments (for example, Horwitz et al., 2003). It should also be recognised that high heritability does not exclude the important role of environment acting through gene-environment interactions (Moffitt et al., 2005).

Linnet and colleagues (2003) completed a systematic review of the evidence for association between prenatal exposure to nicotine, alcohol, caffeine and psychosocial stress. They concluded that exposure to tobacco smoke in utero is associated with an increased risk for ADHD. In contrast contradictory findings were found for the risk from prenatal maternal use of alcohol and no conclusions could be drawn from the use of caffeine. Studies of psychosocial stress indicated possible but inconsistent evidence for an association with ADHD.

Summary

There is consistent evidence of familial influences on ADHD symptoms in the general population. Under the equal environment assumption these familial influences are thought to be largely genetic in origin. Environmental measures associated with ADHD have been identified, the most certain being the association with maternal use of tobacco during pregnancy. It is not known whether these environmental risks represent direct or indirect risks through correlated environmental or genetic factors. Specific genetic variants that are associated with a small increase in the risk for ADHD have been identified in the dopamine D4 and close to the dopamine D5 receptor genes. Analysis of ADHD versus non-ADHD groups has identified consistent changes in brain function and performance on neurocognitive tests; however, differences from controls are not universal, do not characterise all children and adults with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD and do not usually establish causality in individual cases.

1.5. LIMITATIONS

In line with methodology agreed with NICE the approach adopted was initially to identify all available systematic reviews and meta-analytic studies that related to the questions on validity of the diagnosis. While this was possible for much of the neurobiological and genetic and environmental data there were few systematic reviews in other areas such as the factor- or cluster-analytic studies. Where systematic reviews were not available for these studies of ADHD symptoms and studies that investigate the differentiation of ADHD from oppositional and conduct problems, systematic reviews of the primary literature were conducted. For other sub-questions addressed in this section the systematic evidence was supplemented with expert opinion, drawing on evidence known to members of the GDG. The lack of specific reference standards for the diagnosis of ADHD led to an adaptation of the SIGN criteria (see Appendix A) to ensure sufficient quality of the data used to derive recommendations for this guideline.

When considering the Feighner criteria for validity of a psychiatric disorder, the question of whether there are specific responses to clinical, educational and other interventions for ADHD was excluded, since the data to answer this question was very limited. For example it was not possible to identify studies that investigated the effects of stimulant treatments in disorders other than ADHD and there were limited published data on the effects of stimulants in people who do not ADHD [sic]. A paper that did not meet the quality control criteria for the evidence sections of this chapter investigated the response to dexamfetamine and placebo in a group of 14 pre-pubertal boys who did not fulfil criteria for ADHD (Rapoport, 1978). When amphetamine was given, the group showed a marked decrease in motor activity and reaction time and improved performance on cognitive tests. The very small numbers used in this study and lack of further similar studies means that considerable caution must be taken in drawing firm conclusions. Nevertheless, the similarity of the response observed in children without ADHD to that reported in children with ADHD provides further evidence that the aetiological mechanisms that give rise to ADHD are similar to those that influence levels of ADHD symptoms through the population. However the key difference from treatment of people with ADHD is that the ‘behavioural symptoms’ that responded to medication were not causing impairment in the children in this study.

1.6. POSITION STATEMENT ON VALIDITY OF ADHD

Hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity cluster together both in children and in adults and can be recognised as distinct from other symptom clusters, although they frequently co-occur alongside other symptom clusters.

Symptoms of ADHD appear to be on a continuum in the general population.

ADHD is distinguished from the normal range partly by the number and severity of symptoms and partly by the association with significant levels of impairment.

The importance of evaluating impairment and the difficulty in establishing recognised thresholds on the basis of symptom counts alone needs to be addressed. It is not possible to determine a specific number of symptoms at which impairment arises.

There is evidence for psychological, social and educational impairments in both children and adults with ADHD.

ADHD symptoms persist from childhood through to adulthood. In a significant minority, the diagnosis persists and in the majority, sub-clinical symptoms continue to be detectable.

In adults the profile of symptoms may alter with a relative persistence of inattentive symptoms compared to hyperactive-impulsive symptoms.

There is evidence of both genetic and environmental influences in the aetiology of ADHD. It is not known the extent to which there is diversity in the aetiology of the disorder.

Contemporary research suggests that environmental risks are likely to interact with genetic factors, but there is currently limited direct evidence to support this view.

There is evidence of genetic associations with specific genes, environmental risks and neurobiological changes in groups of children with ADHD. However, no neurobiological, genetic or environmental measure is sufficiently predictive to be used as a diagnostic test.

The diagnosis remains a description of behavioural presentation and can only rarely be linked to specific neurobiological or environmental causes in individual cases.

Hyperkinetic disorder (ICD-10) is a narrower and more severe subtype of DSM-IV combined type ADHD. It defines a more pervasive and generally more impairing form of the disorder. Both concepts are useful (Santosh et al., 2005).

There was no evidence of a need to apply a different concept of ADHD to children and adults. However age-related changes in the presentation are recognised.

All current assessment methods have their limitations. There is evidence of the need for flexibility and for a consideration of levels of impairment in assessments and when deriving appropriate diagnoses.

1.7. CONSENSUS CONFERENCE

In addition to a review of published evidence on the question of validity, a consensus conference was held to bring together experts in the field with a range of views, in order to debate the key issues of the use of ADHD as a diagnostic category. The aim was to provide a range of contemporary perspectives that would assist the GDG with the task of deciding what should trigger the use of the guideline and for whom the guideline is intended (see Chapter 3, Methods used to develop this guideline). The speakers delivered a 15-minute presentation addressing the key questions relating to the validity of the ADHD diagnosis set out by the GDG followed by questioning from the GDG members and a subsequent discussion of the presentation among members of the GDG. Each presenter was subsequently asked to provide a summary of their presentation65 and these are also presented in Appendix A.

1.7.1. Discussion on consensus conference presentation

Different presenters brought their own perspectives and this contributed to highlight the importance of a multi-disciplinary approach to the diagnosis and treatment of children with ADHD [sic]. The conference did not consider diagnosis and treatment of adults with ADHD. Here some of the issues that were raised, and the areas of controversy arising from differences in the perception of the different speakers at the consensus conference, are discussed.

The evidence presented at the consensus conference indicated that there was a high degree of unanimity across presenters (coming from a wide range of perspectives) about the fact that there is a group of people who could be seen as having distinct and impairing difficulties and who should trigger the use of this guideline. While recognition of a particular group was agreed upon, uncertainty about the breadth of diagnosis was discussed, namely, whether the use of a narrow (ICD-10 hyperkinetic disorder) versus a broad (DSM-IV ADHD) diagnosis should be used. The problems of using a narrow diagnosis are: (i) the under-recognition of people that are in need of help and (ii) the lack of connection with the research literature, which is based mainly on broader definitions such as DSM-IV. It was established that the main differences between people falling into narrow or broad diagnoses are the breadth of symptoms (requirement for both inattentive, and impulsive-overactive behaviour versus only one domain being sufficient), more or less stringent criteria for situational pervasiveness and the requirement for no major coexisting conditions (apart from oppositional defiant or conduct disorder under ICD-10). Both groups present similar problems of impairment. Overall there was general agreement that both the use of broad (DSM-IV) ADHD diagnosis and narrow hyperkinetic disorder criteria were useful.

One of the major issues of controversy in the UK setting is the very high and variable prevalence rates reported in the literature. For example, recent prevalence figures range from 6.8 to 15.8 for DSM-IV ADHD (Faraone et al., 2003) while the British Child and Mental Health Survey reported a prevalence of 3.6% in male children and less than 1% in female (Ford et al., 2003). Reasons for this are discussed in Faraone and colleagues (2003) who conclude that prevalence rates derived from symptoms counts alone, or from ratings in one setting, were higher than those that took into account functional impairment. For example Wolraich and colleagues (1998) estimated prevalence to be 16.1% on the basis of symptom counts, but 6.8% when functional impairment was taken into account. A study in the UK that specifically addressed the role of impairment found that among 7 to 8 year olds 11.1% had the ADHD syndrome based on symptom count alone (McArdle et al., 2004). In contrast, 6.7% had ADHD with Children’s Global Assessment Scale scores (C-GAS: measuring impairment) less than 71 and 4.2% with C-GAS scores less than 61. When pervasiveness included both parent- and teacher-reported ADHD and the presence of psychosocial impairment prevalence fell lower to 1.4%. The literature on prevalence therefore indicates that the rate of ADHD is sensitive to the degree of impairment associated with the symptom criteria and the degree to which the disorder shows situational pervasiveness.

All the speakers acknowledged the importance of functional impairments in relation to diagnosis, in other words, that diagnostic thresholds should be based on pragmatic grounds such as impairment and the need for treatment. However, there was also agreement that defining suitable thresholds for impairment is difficult due to the breadth of areas in which people with ADHD can be impaired. The level and types of behaviour that define the normal range remain a contentious issue.

On considering when this guideline would be triggered, the GDG concluded that it would be difficult to be prescriptive for any individual case, but that measurement of impairment linked to the symptoms of ADHD is a key component of the decision. Significant problems can arise at various levels, including personal distress from symptoms of the disorder, difficulties in forming stable social relationships and emotional bonds, difficulties with education and long-term risk for negative outcomes such as emotional problems, antisocial behaviour and addiction disorders. The group concluded that treatment response should take into account the severity of the disorder in terms of clinical and functional impairments and evidence should be looked for on the impact of severity of the disorder on treatment response. Overall this is an area in which further research is required to investigate both the short- and long-term outcomes of ADHD and its relation to severity of the condition.

One of the areas of controversy highlighted in the consensus conference was the degree of impairment and severity of ADHD needed to trigger the diagnosis, and related to this, treatment with medication. There is concern in some quarters that the diagnosis automatically leads to treatment with medication and this is not always desirable when the breadth of the definition includes people who might gain substantial benefit from education or psychosocial interventions alone. However even the most ardent supporters of non-medical interventions in ADHD recognised the importance of medical treatment in the most severe cases. In this context the participants in the consensus conference made an important contribution by raising the question of suitable thresholds for ‘significant impairments associated with ADHD symptoms’ and hence the proportion of children fulfilling criteria for the disorder and triggering use of the guideline.

One conclusion is that the acceptable thresholds for impairment are largely driven by the contemporary societal view of what is an acceptable level of deviation from the norm and level of impairment that requires treatment. However the GDG did not consider that the diagnosis should be reserved only for the most serious cases, since the broader concept of ADHD is important in triggering educational and behavioural approaches in addition to medical approaches. The GDG concluded that defining appropriate thresholds of impairment associated with the disorder was important, but that treatment implications might be different for individuals falling above or below particular thresholds.

Confirmatory factor-analytic studies clarify that ADHD symptoms represent a distinct set of symptoms and behaviours that co-vary together in both clinical and control populations. However these cross-sectional studies are far less informative than longitudinal studies that can clarify the predictive outcomes of early ADHD. There are however a few studies that provide suitable data on the relative outcomes of ADHD and other disruptive disorders such as ODD, which are important in delineating specificity in the outcomes related to ADHD. The available evidence suggests that when considering the link between ADHD and conduct problems, ADHD comes first and conduct problems develop later. In contrast there is no evidence that conduct problems in the absence of ADHD lead to the later development of ADHD. The small number of suitable longitudinal outcome studies highlights an important area for future research.

The consensus conference also raised questions about the interpretation of family, twin and adoption studies and the relative contributions between genetic and environmental influences indicated by these studies. The argument against genetic influences is not strong unless one questions the conventional interpretation of twin data. But it is non-controversial that parent and teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms/behaviours show MZ correlations around 70 to 80% and DZ correlations around 20 to 40%; numerous studies replicate this. The usual interpretation of these findings is that the difference in MZ and DZ correlations are mainly the result of genetic influences. The alternative argument that the equal environment assumption is incorrect leads to the conclusion that familial influences are important, but not necessarily genetic. Either way, it is non-controversial that ADHD is familial and this in itself is strong evidence that the construct is sufficiently delineated to show such clear familial effects; that is, that the level of ADHD symptoms in one child strongly predicts the level of ADHD symptoms in his or her siblings. Interestingly there are limited data from twin studies using ADHD cases (for example, concordance rates for the clinical disorder), so the literature mainly uses extremes analysis of rating scale data. Similarly there is a lack of twin data in adult populations.

The GDG agreed that polarised positions in this debate are not helpful since the contemporary understanding of complex behavioural disorders emphasises the importance of interactions between genes and environments. The GDG wishes to stress that the role of important genetic influences does not exclude an important role for environmental influences since individual differences in genetic risk factors are likely to alter the sensitivity of an individual to environmental risks. In this event, reducing environmental risk would be expected to reduce the risk for ADHD. Furthermore, the extent to which there are genetic influences has no direct bearing on the choice of treatment approaches since both medical and psychosocial interventions (or a combination of the two) could be important in improving treatment outcomes.

1.8. EVIDENCE SUMMARY

ADHD should be considered a valid clinical disorder that can be distinguished from co-occurring disorders and the normal spectrum.

ADHD is distinguished from the normal spectrum by the co-occurrence of ADHD symptoms with significant clinical, psychosocial and educational impairments. These impairments should be enduring and occur across multiple settings.

Hyperkinetic disorder is a valid diagnosis that identifies a sub-group of people with ADHD with severe impairment in multiple domains.

ADHD commonly persists throughout childhood and into adult life where it continues to cause considerable psychiatric morbidity.

The quality of the evidence included in this review was variable and lacked any ‘gold standard’ because no diagnostic tests for ADHD have been developed or tested. In the absence of a gold standard for the validity of diagnosis of ADHD or hyperkinetic disorder a lower level of evidence was included in this review.

Although the quality of individual studies included in this review was variable, evidence consistently showed that children and adults with ADHD had associated impairments.

1.9. CLINICAL PRACTICE RECOMMENDATIONS

1.9.1.1.

For the diagnosis of ADHD or hyperkinetic disorder to be made, and for this guideline to be considered appropriate, the following criteria should be met:

  • Symptoms of ADHD (DSM-IV) or hyperkinetic disorder (ICD-10) should be sufficient to reach a formal diagnosis in DSM-IV or ICD-10.
  • ADHD should be considered in all age groups (children, adolescents and adults), with symptom criteria adjusted for age appropriate changes in behaviour.
  • The level of impairment resulting from symptoms of hyperactivity and or inattention should be:

    at least moderately clinically significant on the basis of interview and or direct observation in multiple settings, and

    pervasive (occur in all important settings) including social, familial educational and or occupational settings.

1.9.1.2.

In determining the clinical significance of impairments resulting from the symptoms of ADHD in children, the views of the child should be taken into account, wherever this is possible.

1.9.1.3.

The diagnosis of ADHD should only be made by specialist psychiatrists or paediatricians following a full assessment of the child, adolescent or adult; including all relevant settings.

1.9.1.4.

After making a diagnosis of ADHD or hyperkinetic disorder subsequent assessment and treatment should follow the guideline recommendations.

PART 3. DIAGNOSIS POSITION STATEMENT (PART 1: VALIDITY) PEER REVIEWER CONSULTATION TABLE

Stakeholder

PR – Peer Reviewer

CC – Participant in Consensus Conference

NoTypeStakeholderSectionCommentsReference suggested & reason for inclusion/exclusionActions
20CCDavid Coghill1.1The document uses both the DSM-IV term of ADHD and ICD-10 hyperkinetic disorder. It also, however, uses ADHD as an umbrella term. The GDG should agree on a nomenclature and clarify this at the beginning of the document something along the lines of ‘we will use the terms ADHD (DSM-IV) and hyperkinetic disorder (ICD-10) when talking about the specific diagnostic categories, however when discussing the general disorder we will use ADHD as an umbrella term’ (others have chosen to use ‘AD/HKD’ as the umbrella. To this could be added the paragraph on ‘hyperactivity’ in the last paragraph in Section 1.2.No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.2 ‘ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder’.
78PRJonathan Leo1.1Just because we can diagnose a trait does not mean it is a disease. Your title could leave some people with the mistaken impression that if you can identify a trait and label it, that it can then be called a disease. The validity of the diagnosis – whether you can reliably identify it in some children – is an interesting question, but in this document it is simply a distraction from the main question. The essential question for the NICE committee should be: Is the disease concept of ADHD valid? With that in mind Section 1.4.1, 1.4.2, 1.4.2.1, and 1.4.2.2 have little relevance. The most important Section, which most of my comments address, is 1.4.6.No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.3 and 5.10.
42PRDavid Cottrell1.1This comment may be redundant as definitions may come earlier in the guide but reading this section in isolation I wanted to see a clearer definition of ADHD and HK disorder at the beginning of the chapter.No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.2 ‘ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder’.
21CCDavid Coghill1.2Overall I feel this section needs considerable rewriting as it does not flow at all well. As such does not do justice to the rest of the document which is essentially well written and organised. I have made some suggestions in the text.No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.1 to 5.4.
50PRStephen Faraone1.2You might note that the methodology used to create the Washington University Diagnostic Criteria has been widely accepted and that similar approaches have been used to validate categories for the Research Diagnostic Criteria, the DSM and the ICD criteria (when relevant validating data have been available). My point is that your choice of the WDC is far from arbitrary as there is some consensus as to what the ‘rules of evidence’ should be for asserting the validity of a psychiatric disorder. The intellectual foundation for all these criteria relies heavily on the concept of ‘construct validity’ so well articulated by Paul Meehl decades ago.No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.4.
22CCDavid Coghill1.2 para 2‘Furthermore, in keeping with other common behavioural disorders there is no clear distinction between the clinical condition and the normal variation in the general population’.
The meaning of this sentence is rather unclear. I think it is confusing (or maybe confounding?) symptoms and impairment and to do with the precise words used. There is a continuity of symptoms between those with the disorder and the population. However those with the clinical condition have both high levels of symptoms and impairment leading to a clearer ‘distinction’ between the two. Whilst this may seem trivial the actual sentence is contrary to the conclusions and will be picked up by those who wish to point out that NICE says ‘there is no clear distinction between the clinical condition and the normal variation in the general population’ without clarifying the context of the quotation.
No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.3 (third paragraph) and 5.5.3.
80PRJonathan Leo1.3I think that somehow you need to mention that your literature review was very selective and systematically ignored review articles that were critical of the ADHD diagnosis. As you are aware of both the controversy surrounding the diagnosis and those authors who have addressed the problem, I am assuming that it was a conscious decision to ignore one side of the debate. There is a large body of literature that sees forces other than biology as the source for the dramatic rise in the diagnosis of ADHD. This literature comes from wide and varied sources and is representative of a large segment of the mainstream media and academia. Since one reason that NICE is taking on this difficult task is because the ADHD diagnosis is controversial, it does not make sense to simply ignore critical publications. Just summarising the reviews from mainstream psychiatry journals does not give a balanced view, especially when one considers that it is extremely difficult to get anything published in a psychiatry journal or medical journal that is critical of the ADHD diagnosis. You even acknowledge in one section of the document that, because there was limited data ‘the systematic evidence was supplemented with expert opinion, drawing on evidence known to members of the GDG’.
By selectively choosing data in support of a particular point of view it suggests that your conclusions were made first, and the studies were then subsequently chosen to support your conclusion, and not the other way around, that a group of non-partisan academics analysed all the data and then came to a conclusion.
As just one example of how the debate is framed in academic journals, in 2002, a group of scientists published the International Consensus Statement on ADHD. The consensus statement had several surprising and remarkable declarations such as: ‘Numerous studies of twins demonstrate that family environment makes no significant separate contribution to these traits’ (which runs counter to the NICE document).
‘One gene has recently been reliably demonstrated to be associated with this disorder. . .’
‘Neuroimaging studies of groups with ADHD also demonstrate relatively smaller areas of brain matter.’
‘Most neurological studies find that as a group those with ADHD have less brain activity. . .’
When a group of academicians sent a letter to the editor of Child and Family Psychology Reviews about the Consensus Statement, the editor responded that the letter could be published, but only if Dr. Barkley, the lead author of the Consensus Statement, was given the courtesy of having the last chance to respond. However, this was not a courtesy initially granted to the academicians critical of the rising diagnosis of ADHD, who Barkley and his co-authors compared to members of the flat-earth society. Thus, the authors of the Consensus Statement were given a second chance to cite evidence in support of the biological basis of ADHD, yet rather than cite several specific articles; they instead mentioned that there were hundreds and hundreds of articles. However, good science is not determined by how high the papers can be stacked but by the quality of the papers. To paint those concerned about the rising use of stimulants as somehow on the fringe, shows how isolated academicians can become from the general public. For the GDG to not acknowledge anyone critical of the diagnosis in their own review puts the GDG in the same category as the Consensus authors. Does NICE want to be in the same category?
Nowhere in the GDG document is there a discussion about the ethics of giving a performance-enhancing drug to improve academic success in school - a major reason the drug is used in the first place. For instance, take the announcement about a recent survey, ‘Results of a survey of physicians suggest that parents often request a “behavioral drug”, such as Ritalin, with the goal of enhancing their child’s academic performance rather than treating an illness.’ (Gale, 2006, italics added). The headlines expressed surprise at this practice, yet the practice of prescribing stimulants to improve academic performance is exactly why these medications are prescribed in the first place, and it is fully sanctioned by the medical community. According to Joseph Biederman, ‘If a child is brilliant but is doing OK in school, that child may need treatment, which would result in performing brilliantly in school’ (Gale, 2006). In fact, no official organisation that supports the use of stimulants has ever said that using stimulants to improve academic performance is inappropriate. Even the GDG has not said this is inappropriate. Is it?
References suggested: Gale (2006) Ritalin requests often deemed inappropriate. Medscape.
Paper excluded: relevant to use of Ritalin, not validity of ADHD; not peer reviewed.
References included in the NICE guideline as evidence (found by systematic searches or identified by GDG members) have to meet quality assessment criteria. This is explained in Chapter 3, Methods.

Comments addressed, see Section 5.9 for limitations of references included and last two paragraphs for use of stimulants.

The use of drug treatment (recommendations) is addressed in Chapter 10, Pharmacological treatment.
82PRJonathan Leo1.4In your framing of the question, you ask if environmental factors are associated with ADHD. You then address one review covering the evidence of prenatal exposure to drugs. Again you have systematically ignored a large body of evidence. Perhaps this section is the biggest flaw in your document. Any academic reading this discussion will have a hard time taking you seriously if you cannot think of a single environmental influence coming from the home or school environments that contributes to ADHD. Either you need to comment on this research or explain why you are ignoring it. For instance, a recent study showed that children from divorced families are twice as likely to be diagnosed with ADHD (Strohschein, 2007). And prior studies have shown that children from single family homes are more likely to be diagnosed with ADHD. For more information I have attached two tables and a discussion from Dr Nicky Hart at UCLA who addresses the differences in the ADHD diagnosis across the socioeconomic strata in England and Wales. The data will appear in a forthcoming book, ADHD and Health Inequality.
The statistical evidence generated by the British government as part of its policy making function runs against the impression that ADHD is best thought of as a bio-medical phenomenon. The social distribution of the disorder follows the contours of the class mortality gradient. In other words, it fits the classic profile of health inequality: low prevalence at the top, and high prevalence at the bottom of the social hierarchy. Children exhibiting the symptoms of emotional and conduct disorders, and those afflicted with the troubling symptoms of attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder are much more likely to be poor, to be raised by single and/or unemployed parents, to grow up in neighbourhoods scarred by the signs of under-privilege and to be exposed to stressful life events and social relationships in their early lives.
Figure 1 displays the class gradient of psychiatric morbidity as a whole in British children. The rate is around 4% among children in families where the main breadwinners are employed in higher professional occupations (for example, lawyers, doctors, professors). It is four times higher (16%) in families where parents are either chronically unemployed or have never worked at all.
This group includes single-parent families headed by young women with no labour market experience prior to becoming mothers. The rate of ADHD British style (hyperkinetic disorder) follows the same course. It increases from 0.5% in professional families to 2.6% in households with no attachment to the labour market, a five-fold increase. In between these two poles of social privilege and under-privilege, the risk of mental disorder is around 6% in other middle class strata before ‘jumping’ to more than 8% in the lower supervisory/technical occupations, from this point onwards, it rises steadily on each successive downward rung of the social hierarchy. If we take the lower supervisory occupational category in Figure 1, as the division between the middle (white collar) and working class (blue collar) strata of British society (containing respectively 56 and 44% of the population), we can conclude that social class is strongly associated with children’s mental well-being. Working class kids face a much higher probability of experiencing the symptoms of mental disorder in all its forms than their peers in middle class homes, hyperkinetic disorder is no exception.
The occupational class gradient of ADHD can be translated to another variable representing the social and economic geography of health inequality. This variable is based on the ACORN classification which uses the census characteristics of the area where a child lives (the postal code) to summarise its salient social characteristics. Figure 2, classifies the same sample of children by the quality of their living environment. In a literal sense this variable represents the social and economic environment of daily life and therefore the differential opportunities for physical and intellectual development in childhood.
Once again, we find the social gradient so typical in the health inequality research literature. The symptoms of childhood psychiatric morbidity in areas populated by wealthy families are only half the rate of areas where families with moderate means make their homes. The gap is even wider between the most advantaged and the least disadvantaged neighbourhoods and it apples to all mental disorders as well as hyperkinetic disorder.
References suggested:

Strohschein, L. A. (2007) Prevalence of methylphenidate use among Canadian children following parental divorce. CMAJ: Canadian Medical Association Journal 176, 1711–1714.
Paper included.

Hart, N. (in press) ADHD and Health Inequality. Macmillan.
Paper excluded: not peer-reviewed.
Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.
23CCDavid Coghill1.4 generalI do not think that the whole issue of impairment is dealt with well in this section. It makes a significant appearance in the discussion of the Consensus Conference and in the recommendations however it does not read as if it was critically appraised and considered by the GDG. I think this needs to be remedied within 1.4.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.6.
43PRDavid Cottrell1.4.1First paragraph after subheading ‘Evidence’. I have a problem with the use of the word ‘symptom,’ in this chapter. The issue of the diagnostic validity is a contentious one as illustrated by the lengths the GDG have gone to in consulting widely. ‘Symptom’ implies an illness or disorder about which someone is complaining. Question A in 1.2, repeated at the start of 1.4, and question A1 at the start of 1.4.1 are careful in using the neutral term ‘phenomena’ to describe the behaviours of interest. This seems appropriate given that the whole point of this chapter is to reach conclusions about whether ADHD is or is not a useful construct. To then use the word ‘symptom’ seems to suggest that the issue is already decided. Its use may be appropriate when referring to clinical samples but the use at the end of this paragraph relates to a study where the sample is unclear.
This usage recurs in 1.4.1 and throughout the chapter. For example 1.4.3 has ‘phenomena’ in the title but then refers to ‘continuous distribution of symptoms in the population’ in paragraph 3. I will not list all examples here, and as stated above, ‘symptom’ may be appropriate for research on clinical samples but I think language could be used more carefully and would advocate a word search of the document and consideration on each occasion of the word ‘symptom’ whether it is in fact the best word available.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.2 ‘Symptoms’.
71CCSami Timimi1.4.1(A1) Do the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity cluster together? The GDG conclude, ‘The number of factors varies between studies, with most finding two correlated factors for hyperactivity-impulsivity and inattention; others find that hyperactivity and impulsivity can be distinguished and a few find one combined factor of all three domains’ suggesting little consistency in the literature.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.5.1 and 5.10.
24CCDavid Coghill1.4.1 evidence section (should be 1.4.1.1?)This section would be easier to read if it started with a comment along the lines of, ‘There was strong evidence for clustering of symptoms in both population and clinical samples. Evidence for one, two and three factor models was found with most evidence supporting a two factor model’.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.1 ‘Summary’.
25CCDavid Coghill1.4.1 summaryThe possibility of different patterns across different age ranges should be mentioned in the summaryNo references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.1 ‘Summary’.
44PRDavid Cottrell1.4.2First paragraph, definitions again – it might be helpful to briefly define ‘oppositional defiant and conduct problems’, perhaps in a box? I confess to being unsure what these are myself. Are ‘conduct problems’ the same as conduct disorder? If so why not use a term with an agreed definition? If not we need a definition. The terms oppositional defiant problems, conduct problems, ODD (without ever being given in full), conduct disorder and ‘disruptive behavioural problems’ are all used in Section 1.4.2.1.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.2 ‘Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder’.
72CCSami Timimi1.4.2(A2) Are ADHD symptoms distinguishable from other conditions? It is noted that ‘Frouke and colleagues (2005) conducted a diagnostic study of 2,230 Dutch pre-adolescents from the general population. LCA revealed that ADHD symptoms clustered together with symptoms of oppositional-defiant disorder and conduct disorder. A further study from the Netherlands of disruptive behaviour in 636 7-year-old children (Pol et al., 2003) came to similar conclusions’ and ‘Multivariate twin modelling suggests that while the genetic influences on conduct disorder are largely shared with those that influence ADHD’ and ‘ADHD is reported to co-occur with personality disorder in young offenders (Young et al., 2003)’ and ‘Dysthymia, depression and anxiety symptoms and disorders are frequently associated with ADHD in adults.’ The GDG’s own evidence is suggesting high levels of comorbidity raising doubts about the specificity of ADHD symptoms. The GDG use a ‘get out of jail card’ by concluding that this is because ‘Longitudinal studies suggest that ADHD represents a separate condition that is a risk factor for the development of oppositional and conduct problems.’ However, only one reference is cited in support of this (and this was in a study in which the chair of the GDG is the lead author).No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.2 ‘Summary’ (third paragraph).
62PRAnita Thapar1.4.2.1‘There are in addition environmental factors that influence the risk for conduct problems but not ADHD’. Suggest delete ‘but not ADHD’ Twin studies show important E contribution.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.2.
63PRAnita Thapar1.4.2.1‘The heritability of ADHD symptoms is also higher than that for ODD/CD symptoms in these studies’. Suggest delete that sentence.
It is not scientifically sensible to compare heritability estimates as they are population specific. Also some of the most genetic syndromes (for example, in general medicine) can show lower heritability estimates.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.2.
29CCDavid Coghill1.4.2.1 and sumary for 1.4.2Pervasive developmental disorders are mentioned in the summary but not in the main body of the text. If there is info re. PDD it should be discussed, if there is not this also should be mentioned.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.2 ‘Summary’.
26CCDavid Coghill1.4.2.1 para 2This paragraph should make it clearer that these studies disagree with those cited in para 1 by attaching a statement to that effect before giving the evidence (it is interesting that these are the only two studies in this Section with n reported).No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.2, ‘ADHD and oppositional defiant and conduct problems’.
27CCDavid Coghill1.4.2.1 para 2What is CP?No references suggested.Typing mistake, now reads ‘CD’ for conduct disorder.
28CCDavid Coghill1.4.2.1 para 4‘However the two often occur independently of each other and only partially share aetiological factors’. Should read:
‘However the two often occurred independently of each other and only partially shared aetiological factors’, as it is citing the finding of the study not a general finding.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.2 ‘Summary’.
7PRMargaret Alsop1.4.2.2Due to our involvement within many working and commissioning groups, it has been highlighted by Youth Offending Teams (YOTs), probation services, prisons, young offenders institutes, police, Youth Inclusion Support Programmes (YISPs) Connexions Services, Young People’s Supported Housing, Housing Advice, Young People leaving Care, drug advisory teams and the legal profession such as magistrates/judges that there is now a high percentage of individuals with ADHD or suspected ADHD reaching these services.
According to the Cambridgeshire study in 1995, 90% of recidivist juvenile offenders had a conduct disorder at age 7. Young offenders are now responsible for about a third of all the criminal convictions. A Youth Justice Board survey showed that the number of criminal offences committed by young people is probably far higher than the conviction rates suggest.
References suggested:

Cambridgeshire study (1995).
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.
Comment taken into consideration, see Sections 5.5.2
‘ADHD and oppositional defiant and conduct problems’, ‘ADHD and other co-occurring conditions’ and 5.6 ‘Antisocial behaviour’.
64PRAnita Thapar1.4.2.2‘Overlapping genetic influences on ADHD and conduct problems but the genetic influences estimated by twin studies are greater for ADHD than ODD/CD. . . .’
Delete part of sentence, ‘but the genetic influences estimated by twin studies are greater for ADHD than ODD/CD’. See above for reason.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.2 ‘Summary’.
30CCDavid Coghill1.4.3I feel this section should precede the current 1.4.2 as it would seem logical to sat does adhd separate from normality and if so does it separate from other disorders. [sic]No references suggested.The sequence used follows the Washington University Diagnostic Criteria sequence.
31CCDavid Coghill1.4.3It should be made clearer that the factor approaches can only deal with the symptom level. It does not take into account the whole issue of impairment. Impairment is discussed in some depth in Section 1.7.1 but I feel that it should be discussed or at least better acknowledged in Section 1.4. Again a failure to do so will lead to misuse of isolated sections of the guidance out of context and could lead to misunderstandings.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.3 ‘Summary’ (last paragraph).
51PRStephen Faraone1.4.3I agree with the comments in this section. But one point is missing. I think that the studies which show ADHD to be an extreme of a quantitative trait have typically defined ADHD based on symptom criteria alone. Their results may have been different if impairment criteria were used to define disorder status.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.3 ‘Summary’ (last paragraph).
65PRAnita Thapar1.4.3‘. . .high ADHD symptom scores are the same as those that influence ADHD symptom levels. . .’
DF analysis can’t distinguish this – shows that the magnitude of the heritability estimate is the same for high as for ‘normal range’.
Suggest reword to ‘high ADHD symptoms scores are of the same magnitude as those that influence ADHD symptom levels. . .’
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.3.
73CCSami Timimi1.4.3(A3) Are the phenomena of hyperactivity, inattention and impulsivity distinguishable from the normal spectrum? It is stated that, ‘These studies show that children with ADHD appear to be at one extreme of a quantitative dimension and on this quantitative dimension there is no obvious bi-modality that separates children with ADHD from non-ADHD children.’ It is also noted that ‘there is no obvious threshold or cut-off between ADHD and the continuous distribution of symptoms in the population.’ In the introduction to the document it is stated that ‘in keeping with other common behavioural disorders there is no clear distinction between the clinical condition and the normal variation in the general population.’ The GDG conclude that, ‘Most analytic approaches are unable to make a clear distinction between the diagnosis of ADHD and the continuous distribution of ADHD symptoms in the general population’. In other words the answer to question 1.4.3 is, according to the evidence presented, ‘no’.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.5.3 ‘Summary’.
74CCSami Timimi1.4.4Is the cluster of symptoms that defines ADHD associated with significant clinical and psychosocial impairments? The GDG provides evidence that is consistent with ADHD being associated with significant impairment. However, what is not properly addressed is the nature of this association and direction of causality. For example, with regards academic difficulties it is noted that, ‘These impairments often lead to grade retention (Hinshaw, 2002), to a lower probability of completing schooling when compared with children who do not have ADHD (Mannuzza et al., 1993)’. This association could be mediated by a third factor, such as lowered self-esteem, boy-unfriendly school curricula, frustration, learning difficulties, and so on, that leads to both ADHD symptoms and poor school performance. In family difficulties it is mentioned that, ‘Follow-up studies indicate that mothers of children and adolescents with ADHD have more difficulty in child behaviour management practices and coping with their child’s behaviour (August et al., 1998), and display higher rates of conflict behaviours, such as negative comments, social irritability, hostility and maladaptive levels of communication and involvement (August et al., 1998; Fletcher et al., 1996). Family impairment also permeates the parent’s lives. Parents of children with ADHD report having less time to meet their own needs, fewer close friendships, greater peer rejection, less time for family activities, which might lead to less family cohesion and a significant effect on the parent’s emotional health (Bagwell et al., 2001).’ A vast repertoire of attachment studies also suggest that this association might well indicate important causal factors for ADHD symptoms (that is, these family difficulties cause rather than are caused by ADHD, or more likely interact in varying degrees and combinations depending on the family and individual). With regard to antisocial behaviour the GDG notes, ‘In a prospective follow-up of 103 males diagnosed with ADHD, the presence of an antisocial or conduct disorder almost completely accounted for the increased risk for criminal activities’ and ‘Lee and Hinshaw (2004) reported that the predictive power of ADHD status to adolescent delinquency diminishes when key indices of childhood externalising behaviour related to ADHD are taken into account’. Finally, discussion of long-term outcome is difficult to interpret given that no information is provided by the GDG on the relationship of outcome to other factors known to be associated with poorer outcome such as social class, IQ, comorbid diagnoses and so on.References suggested: attachment studies.
Asked reviewer for full references, no response.
Comment addressed, see Sections 5.6 and 5.11.
32CCDavid Coghill1.4.4 and 1.4.5I think that the substance misuse issues should be considered in 1.4.4 rather than 1.4.5. This Section should also comment on the interactions between early treatment with stimulants and later occurring substance misuse and include nicotine as a drug of misuse that attenuates ADHD symptoms.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.6.
8PRMargaret Alsop1.4.4.2Whilst we agree in principle, our own personal experiences and that of having worked with and supported many families for over a decade through the ADHD Support Group, there are clear indications that parents are still being subjected to accusations of ‘poor parenting’. As parents, not only are we dealing with the family members’ needs, and those of siblings but also with professional bodies such as health, education and social care in which to access an appropriate multi-agency, multi-disciplinary service for the ADHD family member, no one body taking responsibility in which to meet the needs of those with ADHD or those of family members.
Such dealings may lead to conflict between parent carers and service providers, therefore having an impact on service delivery. A high percentage of parents and family members living with ADHD may be accessing mental health services for that of their own needs.
No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
45PRDavid Cottrell1.4.4.2The other parts of 1.4.4 address the potential confounding influence of comorbid conduct disorder, this is not mentioned in this section on family difficulties.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.6 ‘Family difficulties’.
9PRMargaret Alsop1.4.4.3Could it be that by the time the ADHD child is a teenager they may feel that they are somehow different from their friends, but may not understand why? To the ADHD adolescent, they often think that there is some kind of secret code going on between others. This ever widening void is being caused by their inability to learn the code of social cues–those nuances of physical expression and movement that carry half of any conversation and convey personal attitude, varying emotions and defence (or lack of it) between other conversing groups of people.
The ADHD adolescent may have a two-way body language problem involving interpreting others and giving the right signals in return. They may try and copy those around them, without being aware of the subtle complexities or of what is or is not socially acceptable.
‘Too many children fail to achieve their full potential and face involvement in crime, poor health, early unwanted pregnancies, substance misuse or under-achievement in education because services fail to spot the emerging risks or to intervene early enough to coordinate the support necessary. We know that factors such as poverty, failure at school, mental health, family problems or antisocial behaviour can each be possible indicators of future problems’. (Cross Cutting Review of Children at Risk for the 2002 Spending Review).
References suggested: Cross Cutting Review of Children at Risk (2002).
Paper excluded: Not specific to ADHD; not peer reviewed.
See Section 5.6.
10PRMargaret Alsop1.4.4.5Adults with ADHD we have found may have a criminal record of some sort, this highly impacting on their accessing appropriate adult educational programmes, many with no educational qualifications (under-achieving academically), poor record of school attendances or exclusions from education, all of which are contributing factors and play a major role in their employment or future employment status. Multi-agency working to include occupational therapists during transitional services would perhaps contribute towards meeting the needs of those with ADHD and working alongside future employers.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.6 ‘Adolescent and adult problems’.
52PRStephen Faraone1.4.4.5You might also mention the data showing ADHD patients to be at high risk for traffic citations and traffic accidents. You could also mention their increased healthcare utilisation.References suggested:
Risk for traffic accidents.
Asked reviewer for full references, no response.
Comment addressed, see Section 5.6 ‘Adolescent and adult problems’.
11PRMargaret Alsop1.4.5Many children accessing CAMHS or paediatric services may do so until aged 16–17 years. A high percentage of this group may not be referred onto the adult community mental health teams, therefore a child who has received a multi-agency as well as a medicinal approach to treatments for ADHD may well end up in that ‘grey area’ of their not accessing the appropriate healthcare and treatments could all be contributing factors to the possibility of their self-medicating on other substances.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration. For recommendations on this matter refer to the NICE guideline.
46PRDavid Cottrell1.4.5First paragraph after subheading ‘Summary’, line 10 – is ‘appropriate’ correct? Earlier you say the evidence is poor and that developmentally appropriate criteria have yet to be developed. I suspect this should be ‘inappropriate’.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.7.2.
53PRStephen Faraone1.4.5In the summary, the following sentence is not clear and should be re-worded: ‘The profile of symptoms may alter with a relative persistence of inattentive symptoms compared with hyperactive-impulsive symptoms, however the evidence base for this conclusion is poor, using developmentally appropriate measures of hyperactivity-impulsivity in adults.’No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.7.2.
54PRStephen Faraone1.4.5The summary states: ‘There was no evidence to warrant a different diagnostic concept in childhood and in adulthood.’ This seems a bit too strong. The DSM itself allows for a different diagnostic concept:
  1. the category of in partial remission can be used for adults;
  2. a subjective feeling of restlessness can be diagnostic of motor hyperactivity.
Russ Barkley’s new book (and some of his prior work) suggests that the current ADHD symptoms are not developmentally sensitive and there have been some initiatives to re-write the ADHD rating scale (for example, work by Spencer and Adler) so that the questions are more relevant to adults. Also, I think that the greater reduction of hyperactive-impulsive versus inattentive symptoms is more strongly supported than you suggest. But these are all, for sure, debatable points.
References suggested:
Barkley’s book:
Paper excluded:
not peer-reviewed.
Comment addressed, see Section 5.7.2.
55PRStephen Faraone1.4.5I don’t understand the statement, ‘there is a lack of data on the continuity of aetiological factors into adulthood.’ Given that many of the known risk factors for ADHD occur very early in development (for example, genes, fetal toxic exposures), why would we think their effects turn off during adult- hood? I think you mean that we know little about which risk factors modify the course of ADHD through adolescence into adulthood. Probably a re-wording is needed.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.7.2.
75CCSami Timimi1.4.5Is there evidence for a characteristic pattern of developmental changes, or outcomes associated with the symptoms, that define ADHD? It is noted that ‘Faraone and colleagues (2006) analysed data from 32 follow-up studies of children with ADHD into adulthood. Where full criteria for ADHD were used approximately 15% of children were still diagnosed with ADHD at age 25’. This is the only systematic review identified outside of point 1.4.6 (where seven were identified). This finding seems to suggest that ‘characteristic’ outcomes for those diagnosed with ADHD is far from established. Later the GDG speculates as to why rates of substance abuse are higher in those with ADHD symptoms (is this a ‘characteristic’ outcome?) stating: ‘The mechanisms involved can include one or more of the following: first, that individuals with ADHD may seek out highly stimulating or risky activities; second, that individuals with ADHD are exposed to higher levels of psychosocial risks for development of substance use disorders, resulting from educational and social impairments, social exclusion and antisocial behaviour associated with ADHD; third, that various substances, including cannabis, alcohol and stimulants can attenuate ADHD symptoms and are therefore sometimes used as a form of self-treatment.’ Whilst it is unclear why the GDG felt the need to speculate (without evidential references) on this issue, narrow linear biomedical paradigms seems to have allowed them to overlook fairly basic scientific issues. The relationship between ADHD and substance misuse that they are referring to is an association, and thus a third (or more) factor may be responsible for both the substance misuse and ADHD symptoms (such as low self-esteem, family conflict, learning difficulties, comorbid conditions, and so on) making the required criteria of ‘characteristic’ difficult to establish.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.7.2.
33CCDavid Coghill1.4.5 summary‘Using child criteria, approximately 15% of children with ADHD retain the diagnosis by age 25 but a much larger proportion (65%) show persistence of symptoms with associated impairments.’
Could read: ‘Using child criteria, approximately 15% of children with ADHD retain the diagnosis by age 25 but a much larger proportion (65%) show some persistence of symptoms with significant associated impairments.’
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.7.2.
35CCDavid Coghill1.4.6Also the whole issue of heterogeneity at all levels of analysis needs to be discussed as this is central to the whole issue of what is ADHD. I guess the current conclusion would be something like: here are a group of symptoms that hold together pretty well that can be distinguished from normal and other disorders that cause impairment but seem to be the end point (behavioural phenotypic expression) of a wide range of different causal pathways. This would assist the discussion of diversity in Section 1.6.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.
47PRDavid Cottrell1.4.6Could/ should you define executive function for a lay readership?No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.1.
56PRStephen Faraone1.4.6I suggest you include the following: Valera, E. M., Faraone, S. V., Murray, K. E., et al. (2007) Meta- analysis of structural imaging findings in attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 1361–1369.References suggested: Valera, E. M., Faraone, S.V., Murray, K. E., et al. (2007) Meta-analysis of structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 1361–1369. Paper includedComment addressed, see Section 5.8.1 ‘Neuroimaging studies’
57PRStephen Faraone1.4.6A more thorough review of the molecular genetics literature would implicate other genes but that is not essential to make your point. If you’d like to improve the review of environmental risk factors, you could consult: Banerjee, T. D., Middleton, F. & Faraone, S. V. (2007) Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatrica, 6, 1269–1274.References suggested: Banerjee, T. D., Middleton, F. & Faraone, S. V. (2007) Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatrica, 6, 1269–1274. Paper excluded: Review (no systematic search).Comment taken into consideration.
66PRAnita Thapar1.4.6Li et al., 2006:
There have actually been a number of meta-analyses. Most but not all have found the same as Li et al. Might want to mention at least that there have been several. The point of contention is DAT, where most meta-analyses have found no association but some notably have, for example Weiss, S., Tzavara, E. T., Davis, R. J., et al. (2007) Functional alterations of nicotinic neurotransmission in dopamine transporter knock-out mice. Neuropharmacology, 52, 1496–508.
Epub 24 Feb 2007.
References suggested: Weiss, S., Tzavara, E. T., Davis, R. J., et al. (2007) Functional alterations of nicotinic neurotransmission in dopamine transporter knock-out mice. Neuropharmacology, 52, 1496–508. Paper excluded: Only studies on humans considered.Comment taken into consideration.
67PRAnita Thapar1.4.6‘..acting through gene-environment interactions’. Add ‘and gene-environment correlations ()’.
Jaffee, S. R. & Price, T. S. (2007) Gene-environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 432–442. Epub 16 Jan 2007. Review.
References suggested: Jaffee, S. R. & Price, T. S. (2007) Gene-environment correlations: a review of the evidence and implications for prevention of mental illness. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 432–442. Paper excluded: Review (no systematic search).Comment taken into consideration, see Section 5.13.
68PRAnita Thapar1.4.6Literature on prenatal stress? Few studies now on this and ADHD, even though not covered by Linnet study.
Talge, N. M., Neal, C. & Glover, V. (2007) Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why?
The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 245–61. Review.
References suggested: Talge, N. M., Neal, C. & Glover, V. (2007) Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why? The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 245–61. Paper included.Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.1, ‘Physical environ- mental risks’.
76CCSami Timimi1.4.6Is there consistent evidence of genetic, environmental or neurobiological risk factors associated with ADHD? The GDG concludes, ‘Specific genetic variants that are associated with a small increase in the risk for ADHD have been identified in the dopamine D4 and close to the dopamine D5 receptor genes. Analysis of ADHD versus non-ADHD groups has identified consistent changes in brain function and performance on neurocognitive tests; however differences from controls are not universal, do not characterise all children and adults with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD, and do not usually establish causality in individual cases.’ [my italics] The GDG were provided with several papers providing a critical evaluation of research in this area. None were cited in this document.
The GDG after reviewing the evidence and mentioning that, ‘The quality of the evidence included in this review was variable and lacked any “gold standard” ’goes on to recommend that ADHD is valid and to make a diagnosis the following criteria should be met: ‘Symptoms of ADHD (DSM-IV) or hyperkinetic disorder (ICD-10) should be sufficient to reach a formal diagnosis in DSM-IV or ICD-10. ADHD should be considered in all age groups (children, adolescents and adults), with symptom criteria adjusted for age appropriate changes in behaviour. The level of impairment resulting from symptoms of hyperactivity and or inattention should be at least moderately clinically significant on the basis of interview and or direct observation in multiple settings, and pervasive (occur in all important settings) including social, familial educational and or occupational settings.’ This is essentially no different to current DSM-IV criteria and one wonders what the point of this expensive, time consuming exercise was if this is the best the GDG can come up with, particularly when the GDG provide little guidance as to how a clinician is to interpret words like ‘moderately’, and ‘significant’. Given that the chair of the group is well publicised for believing that ADHD is under-diagnosed in the UK, and that using DSM-IV criteria gives prevalence rates of between 3 and 7%, this guideline is likely to result in an increase of ADHD diagnosis. Given these potentially far reaching implications for children and adults in this country and the tenuous evidential support in the document, the basis for the GDG’s conclusions must be questioned.
The GDG states that, ‘It was recognised that defining psychiatric disorders is a difficult process due to the overlapping nature of behavioural and psychiatric syndromes, the complexity of the aetiological processes and the lack of a “gold standard” such as a biological test—in this regard ADHD is no different from other common psychiatric disorders.
Furthermore, in keeping with other common behavioural disorders there is no clear distinction between the clinical condition and the normal variation in the general population’. The phrase ‘two wrongs don’t make a right’ came to mind on reading this. A get-out clause that because other psychiatric diagnoses are problematic constructs (and there is a large literature that attests to this), it is acceptable for a lowering of standards and evidential basis with which to evaluate ADHD, is a circular argument to excuse poor science and insufficient rigour.
The GDG states that, ‘Furthermore, in keeping with other common behavioural disorders there is no clear distinction between the clinical condition and the normal variation in the general population (see Section A3). This is comparable to normal variation for medical traits such as hypertension and type II diabetes’. Such a spurious analogy reveals the extent to which the GDG has ignored one of the most important differences between physical states and psychiatric ones – meaning. 120/80 BP means the same whether it is measured in New York or New Delhi and reflects a physical state (universalism). Further, the pathophysiological processes resulting from high blood pressure are known and independent of the meaning any culture ascribes to symptoms (essentialism). This is not the case for behavioural presentations such as ADHD, which has varying interpretations and meanings, just as beliefs about what is a ‘normal childhood’ and ‘normal child development’ varies enormously over time and between cultures. It is of concern that the GDG seems unaware of the diverse literature (from disciplines such as transcultural psychiatry and psychology, philosophy, anthropology and sociology) criticising the inappropriate use of univeralist and essentialist models (drawn from the biomedical paradigm) in multicultural societies. This is considered a very basic error. Such an approach leads to institutional racism as it assumes that the beliefs and practices about children and childhood drawn from a narrow Western biomedical paradigm is the standard through which to judge those cultures who have differing beliefs and practices with regards their paradigms for understanding the nature of childhood, childhood problems and child care and rearing. This replicates the dynamics of colonialism and such attitudes being promoted for our institutional practices are simply unacceptable in modern multicultural Britain.
The most disappointing aspect of the document is the missed opportunity for a more erudite approach to the question of diagnosis. Given the poor quality of the document it is likely that the current GDG simply does not have the objectivity, knowledge, or sophistication to produce an evidence-based, ethical and progressive review and set of guidelines that could help curtail bad practice, but more importantly provide guidelines that take practice beyond current simplistic paradigms to make it fit for the realities of multi-cultural 21st century Britain. Psychiatry has been increasingly grasping the complexity that comes from a territory that sits at the meeting point of many disciplines’ discourses. Medicine too has increasingly grasped these cross-disciplinary perspectives leading to growth of practices such as narrative medicine and values-based medicine to try and encompass the subjective, cultural, social, political, economic and psychological influences on physical health and medical treatment. In this respect psychiatry should be providing a lead for the rest of medicine as we increasingly move away from redundant dualistic conceptualisations such as mind/body, nature/nurture and universal/relative and toward accepting multiplicity in a way that reflects the diverse nature of the client group we wish to assist. Engagement with these issues would lead to an ability to examine validity of ADHD from a number of angles, recognising that there are many different approaches to this question that reflect different values and aims. For example, in addition to scientific validity, there are considerations of pragmatics, utility, administrative, consistency, relevance, coherence, precision, fecundity, epistemic, ethical, onto-logical, and so on. Using these multiple positions would enable greater transparency and openness to the novel and more flexible guidelines that have greater likelihood of enabling more appropriate engagement with the diverse issues clients with ADHD symptoms present with. It is clear to this author at least, that the current GDG is simply not up to that task.
No specific references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.9.
81PRJonathan Leo1.4.6Comments on D4, D5, DAT1.
Regarding your citation of specific genes involved in ADHD. The evidence is mixed at best. And further-more any connection would only be an association not a cause. Regarding D4 (DRD4) you state there is strong evidence for an association but this is not representative of the scientific literature. According to Willcutt (whom you cite earlier): ‘Similar to the results for DAT1, however, this result [DRD4] was not replicated in all samples and does not appear to be necessary or sufficient to cause ADHD. Moreover, the association with ADHD is much stronger in case-control comparisons than in family-based designs, suggesting that some significant results may be due to differences in gene frequencies in the populations from which the ADHD and comparison samples were drawn’ (Faraone et al., 2001).
In a detailed 2006 survey of the evidence in support of DRD4, DAT1, and other candidate genes, Waldman and Gizer (p. 421) concluded, ‘It should be clear. . .that for each [ADHD] candidate genes studied, there is a mixed picture of positive and negative findings.’ Or as Willcutt stated: ‘For 14 of the 27 candidate genes a significant association with ADHD has been reported in at least one study; however, virtually all of these results have been replicated inconsistently or await independent replication (Table 2). Moreover, each of these genes appears to account for a relatively small proportion of the variance in ADHD symptoms (for example., Faraone, Doyle, Mick, et al., 2001), suggesting that none are likely to be necessary or sufficient to cause ADHD.’ Or as Faraone has stated (2005, p. 1319) with regard to genome wide scans, ‘The handful of genome wide scans that have been conducted thus far show diver-gent findings and are, therefore, not conclusive.’ Regarding your summary of the genetic studies it would be more straightforward to say, ‘At this point in time no genes for ADHD have yet been identified.’
In advertisements for ADHD the supposed genetic basis for ADHD is often used to justify medical treatment. However, left unsaid in these same advertisements, is that a presumed genetic defect is in no way a necessary prerequisite to prescribe stimulants. As of now, the medical community finds it entirely acceptable to prescribe medication for psychological stress brought on by environmental stressors. One needs to look no further than foster care programmes which medicate an inordinate number of children. Presumably the common factor in these children is not their genetic makeup but their common environmental triggers. Although ADHD is considered a genetic defect, looking for a common gene in foster home children to explain their behaviour would seem to be a fruitless effort. Conversely, the results of a survey of environmental stressors in their lives would probably be very fruitful. The diagnosis and medication of children in foster homes is perhaps the best example of how, genes and biology aside, it is an acceptable practice to medicate children whose behaviour is explained by the environment. ‘Although some people question the assumption of the equal environment assumption for identical and non-identical twins this does not impact on the question of validity since the high twin correlations observed in these studies indicates that ADHD symptoms are highly familial.’
This is a very confusing sentence as it mixes up ‘familial’ with ‘genetic.’ It appears to be written by someone who does not understand the genetic studies. The claim that high MZ concordance shows that ADHD is ‘familial’ is erroneous. MZ concordance for speaking Italian is 100%, but does this mean speaking Italian is a genetic trait? The EEA does not just have an impact on estimates of genetic factors–if it is false then the twin method is deeply flawed.
‘There is consistent evidence of familial influences on ADHD symptoms in the general population.
Under the equal environment assumption these familial influences are thought to be largely genetic in origin.’
This is very problematic and should be reworded. The EEA pertains only to the twin method and not to family studies In addition, the document does not provide any citations that the EEA, which is counter intuitive, is correct.
The NICE document also makes the assumption that if it is genetic then it must be a disease. However, a host of other traits have also been investigated and these studies have determined that MZs have a higher concordance than DZs. For instance, twin studies have shown a heritability for loneliness (Boomsma et al. 2005), the frequency of orgasm in women (Daewood et al., 2005), the results of the United States 2004 presidential election (Alford et al., 2005), perfectionism (Tozzi et al., 2004), and breakfast eating patterns (Keski-Rahkonen et al., 2004) (cited in Joseph 2008). In 1990, Bouchard stated: ‘For almost every behavioural trait so far investigated, from reaction time to religiosity an important fraction of the variation among people turns out to be associated with genetic variation. This fact need no longer be subject to debate.’ Yet, if all our traits have a genetic basis then the genetic evidence of a trait does not automatically lead to ‘it’s a disease’ declarations. What many ADHD researchers seem to be saying is, that by implicating genetics in the behavioural trait of attention, this is somehow evidence of a disease. If Bouchard is correct, and all our traits have a strong basis in genetics, then can individuals exhibiting extremes of other traits also fall into the diseased category? The slippery-slope analogy seems almost too obvious to mention here, and might seem trite, however this appears to be exactly the trap that the child psychiatry profession has fallen into regarding other conditions, for instance child-onset bipolar disorder. The NICE statement on ADHD should not be seen with blinders on, as their statements about what constitutes a ‘disease’ will surely be revisited in the years ahead as they face other instances where traits can be classified as a disease in need of medication. NICE’s foray into ADHD is only the beginning.
1.4.6 Dickstein and colleagues completed a systematic meta-analysis of 16 neuroimaging studies that compared patterns of neuroactivity in children and adults with ADHD and controls. The GDG’s position would be stronger if indeed there was a biological marker for ADHD, however to cite the Dickstein paper will be seen as a desperate grasp for evidence. Like much of the ADHD neuroimaging research, on the surface the Dickstein paper might appear to make the case that there is a visible organic pathology in the brains of children diagnosed with ADHD, however a more in-depth view of the study reveals problems of experimental design that have plagued this entire body of research. The Dickstein paper is a meta-analysis of 16 ADHD imaging studies. Out of these 16 studies, four were used for a comparison of ADHD non-medicated to controls – the most important comparison. Interestingly, Dickstein et al. do not mention that two out of these four studies used the same ADHD subjects – the two studies were separate papers but came from the same research group. This is fairly obvious from reading the two studies, and was confirmed in an email to the lead author. Emails to the corresponding author of the Dickstein paper asking for clarification have gone unanswered. Double counting subjects is problematic for a meta-analysis, especially since Dickstein et al. did not mention it in their paper. Furthermore, even in the Dickstein analysis for the most important comparison in the study which was the non-medicated ADHD to controls the majority of the differences were for the most part not significant. It seems highly problematic for the NICE review to not mention this in their review.
Although positive findings on neuroimaging studies of psychiatric disorders, including ADHD, are usually given wide coverage in scientific publications and the mass media, the fact remains that this body of research has not provided support for a specific ‘biological basis’ of ADHD. This is well noted by Baumeister and Hawkins (2001) who report, ‘inconsistencies among studies raise questions about the reliability of the findings’ (p. 2). Writing, for instance, about the tendency for studies to find decreases in the size and activity of the frontal lobes, Baumeister and Hawkins summarise that:
‘Even in this instance, however, the data are not compelling. The number of independent replications is small, and the validity of reported effects is compromised by a lack of statistical rigor. For example, several of the major functional imaging studies failed to employ standard statistical controls for multiple comparisons. This means that many of the reported findings are almost certainly spurious. Moreover, considering the likely existence of bias toward reporting and publishing positive results, the literature probably overestimates the occurrence of significant differences between subjects with ADHD and control subjects’ (p. 8, references omitted).
In addition, virtually all researchers in this field acknowledge that no brain scan can currently detect anomalies in any given individual diagnosed with a primary mental disorder, nor can it help clinicians to confirm such a diagnose. For example, in his authoritative Handbook of Brain Imaging, Bremmer (2005) states:
‘Unfortunately, we are not at a point where brain imaging can be used routinely for the diagnosis of psychiatric conditions. . . . We still do not understand the pathophysiology or mechanisms of response to treatment for most of these disorders. . .Most studies of psychiatric patients have found that even when a particular finding characterized a patient group, there remained as many as a third of patients who scored in the range of the control subjects’ (pp. 33–35).
Similarly, in the case of ADHD, Giedd and colleagues (2001) conclude unequivocally that: ‘If a child has no symptoms of ADHD but a brain scan consistent with what is found in groups of ADHD, treatment for ADHD is not indicated. Therefore, at the time of this writing, clinical history remains the gold standard of ADHD diagnosis’ (p. 45).
The Dickstein paper was funded by NIMH. Of interest to the NICE reviewers might be the 2003 paper by Sowell and colleagues also funded by NIMH. The Sowell study, involving 27 ADHD and 46 normal control subjects, reported that ADHD children had smaller frontal lobes compared to the control subjects, but overall the ADHD subjects had more cortical grey matter (Sowell et al., 2003). This study’s significance derives not necessarily from this result, but – as with several previous ADHD neuroimaging studies – from important comparisons that researchers could have made, but did not. One reason for bringing this study to your attention is because of your own acknowledgment in your previous reports about other conditions and treatments (the SSRIs for instance) that seeing the published data is not the same as seeing all the data, because the pharmaceutical companies do not publish all their studies. The same holds true for research into basic science topics, although in this case it is government-funded organisations that will not release data.
As in an earlier, similar paper by Castellanos and colleagues (2002), some of the ADHD subjects in the Sowell study were apparently medication-naïve. I say ‘apparently’ because specific descriptions were not provided: ‘15 of the 27 patients were taking stimulant medication at the time of imaging’ (p. 1705). It is unclear how to categorise the remaining 12 patients. Did they have a history of medication use prior to the start of the study, and then stop taking their medication for 48 hours, or some other arbitrary time period before imaging? It is surprising that a study published in The Lancet could be so vague about one of the most important variables in the study. Conclusions based on a comparison of normal control subjects to medication-naïve ADHD subjects would be very different from conclusions based on a comparison of control subjects to ADHD subjects with varying durations of medication exposure or undergoing abrupt withdrawal.
The issue becomes considerably more muddled and confusing due to a brief concluding discussion by Sowell and colleagues (2003) of the potential role of stimulant medication on their findings. The authors first appropriately acknowledged that, since 55% of their ADHD children were taking stimulants, ‘the effects of stimulant drugs could have confounded our findings of abnormal brain morphology in children with [ADHD]’ (p. 1705). The simplest way to properly evaluate this confounding effect would have been to compare the 15 medicated ADHD children with the 12 unmedicated ADHD children. However, Sowell and colleagues chose to not make that comparison: ‘We did not directly compare brain morphology across groups of patients on and off drugs because the sample size was considerably compromised when taking lifetime history of stimulant drugs into account’ (p.1705). The authors further explained that this comparison, between unmedicated and medicated, is not needed because a prior study by Castellanos and colleagues (2002) suggested that medications do not affect brain size.
Sowell and colleagues’ methodological choice, and its justification, is both unconvincing and puzzling. First, although one can sympathise with their judgement that ‘taking lifetime history of stimulant medication into account’ compromised their sample size, this judgement ignores that for 30 years ADHD neuroimaging researchers have deemed it perfectly acceptable to compare ADHD subjects and normal controls regardless of medication history. Indeed, virtually all the studies that Sowell and colleagues cite to contextualise their own study and interpret their results exemplify this practice. Thus it is difficult to see why Sowell and colleagues would feel that they should not compare medicated and unmedicated ADHD subjects. Clearly, just as they acknowledged limitations to their main study results, Sowell and colleagues could have reported the results of the more specific comparison with an acknowledgement of the appropriate limitations. Second, Sowell and colleagues cite Castellanos and colleagues to support the methodological choice of not comparing medicated and unmedicated ADHD subjects. But, third and most important, Sowell and colleagues’ data appear directly relevant to either support or refute the conclusions that Castellanos and colleagues (2002) drew from their comparison. In fact, the results of the Castellanos and colleagues’ comparison of brain volumes of medicated and unmedicated ADHD children were deemed worthy of a major press release by the NIMH concerning stimulant drugs’ effects on developing brains, yet the same comparison in the Sowell and colleagues study was considered insignificant and not even reportable.
Sowell and colleagues would not supply the information about the most important comparison in the study, and a subsequent Freedom of Information Act Request to NIMH to release the information was denied. This was in spite of the fact that on their own website NIMH encourages their grant recipients to share data. One could say that NIMH’s actions speak louder than their words. Given their own interest in the subject, possibly the NICE reviewers could request the data?
In June 2006, the American Journal of Psychiatry published three articles (Pliszka et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2006; Tamm et al., 2006) and an accompanying editorial about functional magnetic resonance imaging (Casey & Durston, 2006). The three studies conducted scans of children’s brains during a specified task, and, importantly, all three studies had a group of medication-naïve ADHD children. However, when considered together, the three studies implicated an inordinate number of different brain regions, with little replication of the regions between studies. In brief, Smith and colleagues (2006) implicated the frontal, parietal, and temporal lobes, along with the striatum. Pliszka and colleagues (2006) implicated the anterior cingulate cortex and the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Tamm and colleagues (2006) implicated the parietal lobes, the right precuneus, and the thalamus. One could almost ask: What area of the brain is not implicated?
The accompanying editorial by Casey and Durston (2006) acknowledges these disparate findings, yet instead of looking at them as problematic for the ADHD neuroimaging field, Casey and Durston attempt to place the disparate findings within a theoretical construct that cognitive deficits in ADHD are due to a deficit in inhibitory control. They state: ‘Identification of core processes involved in a disorder can move a field from a disparate set of data-driven findings to a more theoretically coherent collection of studies’ (p. 957). Does Casey and Durston’s model provide a solid base for ADHD researchers to move forward, or is their explanation of these ‘disparate findings’ an attempt at salvaging a lack of reproducibility within the ADHD neuroimaging field? The model as proposed by Casey and Durston is that, ‘basic learning systems are important in signalling top-down systems to adjust behaviour when predicted outcomes are violated.’ This appears to be little more than a very general statement about learning. As a general statement it is hard to argue with it, because it is so broad and all-encompassing that it makes room for almost every conceivable finding. But it does little to explain how upwards of 10–15% of the population has a disease. One test for whether a theory is too broad, is to ask: What empirical findings would negate the theory? Casey and Durston have not proposed any findings that would negate their theory, and, indeed, it is hard to imagine any that would negate it. For instance, in Figure 16 of their article, Casey and Durston hypothesise the involvement of the prefrontal cortex, the basal ganglia, the parietal cortex, and the cerebellum in ADHD. Yet none of the three accompanying studies even suggested that the cerebellum was involved. Bringing the cerebellum into the picture without elaboration is also problematic because as Furman notes: ‘of the five studies that examined total cerebellar volume, four are listed as showing an association of ADHD with decreased volume, while three do not.’ And missing from Casey and Durston’s schematic is the thalamus, which one study did implicate.
Moreover, two of the studies were contradictory: Pliszka and colleagues found greater activity in ADHD subjects than controls in the inferior prefrontal cortex (p. 1059), while Smith and colleagues found less activity (underactiviation) in the mesial and front-parietal-temporal brain regions during the go/no go and switch tasks for the ADHD children. Yet, interestingly, while the imaging data for the ADHD children differed in these two studies, there was no difference in performance on the specified tasks between the ADHD children and controls. None of these issues are raised by Casey and Durston, and we are unsure how they could be fitted into the proposed model.
Perhaps the most significant aspect of putting forth such a highly theoretical model of ADHD is that Casey and Durston are implicitly acknowledging that the more practical aspect of developing an imaging scan as a diagnostic tool is becoming more and more unlikely.
A recent study by Volkow and colleagues (2007) utilised PET and compared dopamine transporter levels in 20 never medicated adults to 25 controls, and found that dopamine transporter levels were not positively correlated with the disease. In the NICE document, to your credit, you have few positive statements about this research, but on the other hand you do not come right out and acknowledge this. For instance, Volkow, in much more direct terms than NICE, has commented: ‘it should be noted that the imaging studies are still not definitive because of the discrepancies in the findings.’
The necessary and definitive test to confirm the suggestion that ADHD children have a neuroanatomic pathology consists of using an appropriate brain scan to detect a difference between a ‘typical’ unmedicated ADHD child as found in the classroom, and a ‘normal’ child. There is virtual unanimity that this cannot be accomplished at present. Experiments with highly selective patient and control groups are, at best, only preliminary studies, and the findings of these studies must be called into question. Ruling out the effects of psychotropic medication is merely one of the tasks confronting researchers conducting neuroimaging research with ADHD patients. Even if the field accomplishes this task, however, several other important tasks remain. One of these will involve trying to make sense of findings of brain abnormalities or differences among some individuals diagnosed with ADHD. In October 2005, for example, the New York Times published an article by Benedict Carey entitled ‘Can brain scans see depression?’ It contained interviews with prominent psychiatrists and child psychiatrists, many of whom have authored ADHD imaging papers. The Times article was notable for both its candour and frank assessment of the psychiatric neuroimaging field: ‘Yet, for a variety of reasons, the hopes and claims for brain imaging in psychiatry have far outpaced the science, experts say.’ And in the words of Paul Wolpe, a professor of psychiatry and sociology: ‘The thing for people to understand is that right now the only thing imaging can tell you is whether you have a brain tumor.’ A recent imaging study found a difference between the brains of conservatives and liberals. Does this difference equate to a disease?
References suggested: Waldman (2006)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Baumeister & Hawkins (2001)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Giedd (2001)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Sowell (2003)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Castellanos (2002)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Pliszka, S. R., Glahn, D. C., Semrud-Clikeman, M., et al. (2006) Neuroimaging of inhibitory control areas in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder who were treatment naive or in long-term treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1052–1060. Paper included.

Smith (2006)
Am J Psychiatry, 163, 1033–1043.
Paper to be included.

Tamm (2006)
Am J Psychiatry, 163, 1033–1043.
Paper to be included.

Casey & Durston (2006)
Am J Psychiatry, 163, 1033–1043. Paper excluded: not peer reviewed (editorial).

Volkow (2007)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Carey (2005) Can brain scans see depression? The New York Times. Paper excluded: not peer reviewed; opinion paper.
Comment addressed, see Appendix 17.1
‘Study characteristics – Diagnosis’ for information on funding of studies included, see Section 5.8
‘Neuroimaging studies’ for discussion of genetic studies.
34CCDavid Coghill1.4.6 evidenceI think this section would read better if it were reordered to deal with causal factors, that is genetic, environmental and then mediating factors. In addition to the mediating factors already discussed (functional imaging, neuropsychology) structural imaging and neurophysiology should be added).
The neuropsychology section stresses executive functions too strongly (although these are the most well studied other functions like delay aversion [Sonuga-Barke], timing [Tannock & Smith], non-executive memory [Rhodes & Coghill] and as noted variability also contribute – and may actually prove to be more important than executive functions).
On the other hand the statement, ‘Recently it has emerged that the strongest and most consistent association with ADHD is for intra-individual variability (Klein et al., 2006)’ is way too strong as it relies on only one study. A similar argument could be made for a range of different neuropsychological functions based on other comparative studies.
References suggested: Sonuga-Barke
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Tannock & Smith Asked reviewer for full reference, no response.

Rhodes & Coghill Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.
Comment taken into consideration, see Section 5.8.
36CCDavid Coghill1.5Whilst there are not many child studies where healthy kids or kids with other disorders have been given methylphenidate or dexamfetamine there are many such adult studies. My understanding is that these support the notion that these stimulants in these doses work the same in healthy people as they do in those with problems.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.9 (third paragraph).
58PRStephen Faraone1.5You state: ‘When considering the Feighner criteria for validity of a psychiatric disorder, the question of whether there are specific responses to clinical, educational and other interventions for ADHD was excluded, since the data to answer this question was very limited.’ I don’t have the Feighner criteria in front of me but I thought that the idea was that the disorder showed a ‘characteristic’ response to treatment rather than a ‘specific’ response. For example, the fact the SSRIs treat depression, OCD and other anxiety disorders does not challenge the validity of any of these disorders.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.8.
84PRJonathan Leo1.5Limitations. When discussing the effect of stimulants on people not diagnosed with ADHD, regarding the Rapoport study, you state, ‘there were limited published data on the effects of stimulants in people who do not have ADHD.’
This is an incredible statement as it seems to be saying that we do not know the effect of stimulants on normal people. Underlying any discussion of ADHD (except for possibly the NICE document) and what every neuroscience researcher is aware of, is the understanding that the most straightforward experiment in all of neuroscience is the one seeking to determine if stimulant medication works, at least if one defines ‘works’ as a short-term improvement in attention span. Whether the subjects are male or female, whether they are pre-schoolers or geriatrics, whether they are diagnosed with ADHD or not, and whether the medication is provided by a doctor or a friend, it has been known for 75 years that stimulants improve anyone’s and everyone’s ability to pay attention.
The GDG sidesteps the issue of the fact that the stimulants such as methylphenidate (Ritalin) have a universal effect by stating that they are going to discuss the treatment of ADHD in a subsequent document. (However, even this is problematic because the document brings up the Rapoport study at one point.) Talking about treatment with stimulants in a future document is fine, but while it might be convenient this does not justify, in the current document, ignoring the universal effect of the stimulants on the CNS – as this does relate to the disease concept. Unfortunately much of the press still falls back on the so-called ‘paradoxical effect’ that sees stimulants only effecting ADHD children. Rapoport’s study shows this is false. Coffee drinkers also know this is false. Apparently one of the few organisations to not acknowledge this fact is the GDG.
Also regarding the Rapoport study, you state, ‘The very small numbers used in this study and lack of further similar studies means that considerable caution must be taken in drawing firm conclusions.’
Again, your double standard is evident. The NICE review suggests ‘considerable caution’ when drawing conclusions about a study looking at the effect of amphetamines on the normal brain. Yet, just one page before in the review, there seems to be no hesitation or ‘caution’ in your interpretation of the genetic studies, which have not discovered any ADHD genes, or the imaging studies, which are unable to distinguish ADHD children from controls.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.9.
13PRMargaret Alsop1.6It is felt that the evidence submitted by parents, carers and others caring for an individual diagnosed as having ADHD is clear evidence on the validity of ADHD. There seems to be clear indication that the evidence submitted by professionals and those within the GDG echoes that of parents, carers and individuals themselves.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
37CCDavid Coghill1.6The comment that, ‘In adults the profile of symptoms may alter with a relative persistence of inattentive symptoms compared to hyperactive-impulsive symptoms’ does not really match up with the evidence described in Section 1.4.5 where it is suggested that evidence for this is weak and that relative to controls levels of overactivity stay high. Here would be a good place to dispel this notion of a true reduction in overactivity problems as one of the ADHD myths.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
38CCDavid Coghill1.6‘There was no evidence of a need to apply a different concept of ADHD to children and adults. However age-related changes in the presentation are recognised.’ Could be expanded to add: ‘There was no evidence of a need to apply a different concept of ADHD to children and adults. However age-related changes in the presentation are recognised. These changes are not yet reflected within the various diagnostic criteria.’.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.12.
83PRJonathan Leo1.6Position Statement on the Validity of ADHD. ‘There is evidence of both genetic and environmental influences in the aetiology of ADHD. . . . .Contemporary research suggests that environmental risks are likely to interact with genetic factors. . . .’ Why is that whenever environmental influences are brought up that you feel the need to drop genetics into the discussion? When you say ‘contemporary research suggests that environmental risks are likely to interact with genetic factors’ what recent research are you referring to? You are making it sound like the ADHD genetic researchers have recently uncovered this startling fact. However, the fact that genes interact with the environment has been known for years.
According to Robert Sapolsky, ‘Genes influence behaviour, the environment influences behaviour, and genes and environment interact – this view is one of the great scientific clichés of the 20th century.’
Commenting on the usefulness of the ‘vulnerability – stress theory of mental disorders’ that any potential harmful environmental influences only operate on those with faulty genes, Mary Boyle points out that the theory is an important mechanism for managing the potential threat posed to biological psychiatrists whenever non-biological conditions are implicated in the aetiology of psychological stress. The usefulness of the hypothesis lies partly in its lack of specificity – since the nature of the claimed vulnerability has never been discovered, anything can count as an instance of it. Its usefulness also lies in its seeming reasonableness (who could deny that biological and psychological or social factors interact?) and its inclusiveness (it encompasses both the biological and social – surely better than focusing on only one?) while at the same time it firmly maintains the primacy of biology, not least through word order, and potentially de-emphasises the environment by making it look as if the ‘stress’ part of the vulnerability – stress model consists of ordinary stresses which most of us would cope with, but which over-whelm only ‘vulnerable’ people. We are thus excused from examining too closely either the events them-selves or their meaning to the ‘vulnerable’ person (Boyle, 2002).
Your document seems to be the perfect example of what Boyle is referring to. You maintain the primacy of biology with your wording, but as Boyle points out, the driving force behind your wording is not ‘contemporary research’ that has discovered an ADHD gene, but is the contrast between the genetic studies, that have failed to find a specific ADHD gene or even a gene of modest effect, and studies implicating environmental factors. If you had discovered a gene, then you would not be talking about the vulnerability-stress hypothesis. Again, take the example of foster care homes where an inordinate number of children are diagnosed with ADHD (and other conditions). Clearly this data points to environmental influences on ADHD, no matter what genes a child is born with.
As the data from Nicky Hart shows, there appears to be a strong role for socioeconomic strata. If we follow your logic, then the increased prevalence of children with smaller brains and less electrical activity (according to the current concept of ADHD) in the lower socioeconomic strata must be qualified with the statement that their smaller brains are due to faulty genes being influenced by the environment.
No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
88PRJonathan Leo1.6Position Statement on Validity of ADHD. ‘ADHD is distinguished from the normal range partly by the number and severity of symptoms and partly by the association with significant levels of impairment.’
Your statement points out why the diagnosis varies so much from one country to another, from one doctor’s practice to another, from one school to another, and from one household to another. Take the 2004 guidelines on the diagnosis of ADHD from the American Academy of Pediatricians. Take item 2 on their questionnaire as an example:
2)

Six (or more) of the following symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity have persisted for at least 6 months to a degree that is maladaptive and inconsistent with developmental level:

Hyperactivity
  1. Often fidgets with hands or feet or squirms in seat
  2. Often leaves seat in classroom or in other situations in which remaining seated is expected
  3. Often runs about or climbs excessively in situations in which it is inappropriate (in adolescents or adults, may be limited to subjective feelings of restlessness)
  4. Often had difficulty playing or engaging in leisure activities quietly
  5. Is often ‘on the go’ or often acts as if ‘driven by a motor’
  6. Often talks excessively
Note that every item on the list uses the term ‘often’, a very unscientific term. How does one quantify ‘often’. Since ‘often’ is in the eye of the beholder and can vary from one doctor’s office to the next it is easy to see how this document has little teeth to it.
Apparently as long as one adult decides that the child ‘often’ fidgets, then the child can be labelled and medicated. It is easy to see how a parent who does not get a diagnosis from one doctor can simply go to another doctor with different ideas about what ‘often’ means. As an example of how the general public sees through a document like this take this example provided by the late Kevin McCready (2002): ‘In an episode of The Sopranos, the popular and critically acclaimed HBO series about a New Jersey mobster and his family, the primary character, Tony Soprano, is called into a meeting with school officials, including the school psychologist. Tony is told that his son has been determined to “have” ADHD. He asks how this has been determined and is told there is a set of criteria, which the psychologist then begins to itemise. The third criterion on the list is “tends to fidget.” The poorly educated, psychologically unsophisticated, working class gangster looks at directly at the psychologist and asks simply in his earthy “Jersey” accent: “What constitutes a fidget?” There may be little to admire about a man who makes his living illegally, but at least he “gets it.”‘.
It is easy to see how guidelines that use the word ‘often’ mean very little. Is NICE going to develop more stringent guidelines?
No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
39CCDavid Coghill1.7.1The term ‘medical treatment’ should be replaced by ‘pharmacological treatment’ or ‘drug treatment’.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.12.
59PRStephen Faraone1.7.1I don’t understand the following sentence: ‘The group concluded that treatment response should take into account the severity of the disorder in terms of clinical and functional impairments and evidence should be looked for on the impact of severity of the disorder on treatment response.’No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.13.1.
69PRAnita Thapar1.7.1‘The argument against genetic influences is not strong unless one questions the conventional interpretation of twin data’ and continuing paragraph. Contribution of genetic influences doesn’t rest purely on twin studies of ADHD. There have been five adoption studies all showing familial clustering due to genetic influences (refer to any review on ADHD genetics).
Thapar, A., Langley, K., Owen, M. J., et al. (2007) Advances in genetic findings on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 17, 1–12.
Khan, S. A. & Faraone, S. V. (2006) The genetics of ADHD: a literature review of 2005. Current Psychiatry Reports, 8, 393–397. Review.
References suggested: Thapar, A., Langley, K., Owen, M. J., et al. (2007) Advances in genetic findings on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 17, 1–12. Paper excluded: Review (no systematic search).

Khan, S. A. & Faraone, S. V. (2006) The genetics of ADHD: a literature review of 2005. Current Psychiatry Reports, 8, 393–397.
Paper excluded: Review (no systematic search).

References of individual studies were hand searched and those meeting the quality assessment criteria were included [Sprich et al., 2000].
Comment addressed, see Section 5.8 and throughout the chapter.
85PRJonathan Leo1.7.1‘The level and types of behaviour that define the normal range remain a contentious issue.’
The current GDG believes that children with ADHD have an organic brain deficiency, resulting from a genetic defect that in the future, once the technology is available, will be detected by a brain scan. However nowhere in the document has NICE answered the controversial question: How many children have this defect? The obvious problem being that as the percentage of children taking Ritalin escalates, the harder it is to make the case that they have a disease. If, as in some school districts, upwards of 17% of the boys are prescribed Ritalin, this would suggest that the boundaries for normalcy have become narrower. As often happens after statistics documenting the increasing use of stimulants for younger and younger children make the headlines, many of the opinion leaders in the psychiatry community state that there is a problem with ‘over-prescribing’ or ‘misdiagnosis’, yet none of these leaders, or any of the major psychiatric organisations, has issued guidelines on how to identify this large group of ‘misdiagnosed’ children, nor have they clarified what they consider to be improper uses of prescribed stimulant medication (Johnson, 2006; Nakamura, 2002). No matter where NICE draws the line between normal and ADHD, whether it classifies 2%, 5% or 7% as having the disease, there will, by definition, be children who are inappropriately taking stimulant medication. Based on what criteria will NICE decide who these misdiagnosed children are? For instance, if according to NICE 7% of British children have ADHD then what if 10% are taking medication? How will doctors identify the 3% of misdiagnosed children?
The dilemma for medical professionals who want to go beyond simply talking about misdiagnosed children and to actually identifying these children is that, without an objective biological marker demarcating the line between the ‘correctly’ and ‘incorrectly’ diagnosed, the sole criterion for determining the appropriateness of stimulant treatment comes down to: Are the adults in the child’s life satisfied with the medication’s effect? Presumably there are not many parents unhappy with the medication’s effects, who still continue to medicate their children.

None of the medical professionals who talk about misdiagnosis has ever elaborated on how they plan to tell all these parents of misdiagnosed children that they should not be medicating their children, even though the medication is doing exactly what the medical community says it should be doing.
As an example of the forces at work in the diagnosis of an individual child with ADHD, take a case study in the journal Pediatrics. In 1999, the editors elicited commentaries from several prominent physicians about the case of a teenage boy who had been taking Ritalin for several years. The editors saw the boy’s scenario as an interesting case, worthy of commentary from a group of prominent child psychiatrists. But in an ironic twist of fate, they have unintentionally provided a much more interesting case study. From a sociological point of view the subject of the case was not the boy, but, instead, was the doctors and the editors. The case provides an excellent example of:
  1. how a major determination in the diagnosis of ADHD is adult satisfaction,
  2. how the medical community fully supports the use of stimulant medication as a performance enhancing drug,
  3. how the same mindset that approves of using one psychotropic drug easily leads to the use of multiple medications and
  4. how the main stream medical journals have given little attention to the ethical implications of controlling and altering children to meet the demands of our contemporary educational/cultural system.
‘The 15-year-old boy announced to his parents and his pediatrician that he wanted to stop taking his medication: “I don’t need it. . .I’m fine. . .I don’t see why I should take it.” He purposefully did not take the medication for a few weeks and he said he could not tell the difference. . .. However, his parents observed that his test results, when off the medication, were below his standard scores. . .. They also noted that he was more distractible and less attentive when doing his homework during that time’ (Cohen & Leo, 2002).
As stated by the physicians, the most important variable in determining whether this boy should keep taking his medication was the parental satisfaction with the medication, and the subsequent commentaries all focused on how to convince the boy to continue taking his medication. The boy’s wishes were not something to be listened to, but rather something to be managed, whether through dialogue or with another medication. As an example of polypharmaceuticals for children one of the commentators even suggested that the boy’s reluctance to keep taking his Ritalin suggested this was a sign that he needed another medication. Thus the boy, who wants go off his one medication, would instead get two medications. None of the commentators in the Pediatrics article contemplated that the boy’s wishes might be legitimate, but more importantly, as a sign of how one-sided the issue has become in the medical community, the editors did not give space to a single commentator who questioned the ethics of giving a medication to improve grades.
As an example of who the experts in America are diagnosing with ADHD take this example from the Department of Psychiatry at New York University: ‘Sarah chooses to sit in the back of the classroom and much of the time she’s doodling in her notebook or staring out of the window. She seldom completes assignments and often forgets to bring the right book to class. Her desk is a mess and she generally can’t find what she is looking for. Then she gets weepy and says that nobody understands her.’ This 14-year-old girl is crying out ‘please understand me’. The New York University experts’ response is to label her with ADHD. Medication will surely follow. Examples like this and the others I have cited, which come from those who strongly believe that ADHD is biological, are just more examples of how little science is involved in the ADHD diagnosis.
References suggested:

Johnson (2006)
Study: ADHD drugs send thousands to ERs.
Paper excluded: not peer reviewed.

Nakamura (2002) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorders: are children being overmedicated?
NIMH.
Paper excluded: not peer reviewed; opinion paper.

Case study & editor comments, Pediatrics (1999)
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.
Comments taken into consideration.
86PRJonathan Leo1.7.1‘The GDG wish to stress that the role of important genetic influences does not exclude an important role for environmental influences since individual differences in genetic risk factors are likely to alter the sensitivity of an individual to environmental risks.’ This is confusing because earlier in the document the only environmental influences that you mentioned were prenatal exposure to drugs such as nicotine. What environmental influences are you referring to here? ‘Furthermore, the extent to which there are genetic influences has no direct bearing on the choice of treatment approaches since both medical and psychosocial interventions could be important in improving treatment outcomes.’ Let’s be honest here, the idea that ADHD is due to genetics is one of the most common reasons cited by the pharmaceutical companies and the psychiatric profession as evidence that ADHD is a biological disease, like diabetes. And diseases are treated with medications. Your statement is even going against much of modern day psychiatry. According to Nancy Andreasen, in The Broken Brain, the biological model of mental illness can be summed up as follows:
1.

The major psychiatric illnesses are diseases,

2.

These diseases are caused principally by biological factors and most of these reside in the brain,. . .and

4.

The treatment of these diseases emphasises the use of somatic therapies.’

As another example of this type of thinking, take the recent comments by Stephen Faraone in Science. In a discussion of ‘ADHD genes’ he stated: ‘My hope is that once we’ve discovered those genes, we’ll be able to do a prospective study of kids at high versus low genetic risk. That’s when you’ll see environmental factors at work.’ But certainly one can still see environmental factors at work in children without knowing their genotype. Yet, even more confusing is Faraone’s next comment. According to the reporter, ‘Eventually, he [Faraone] adds, environmental changes could play an important role in treating some ADHD patients’ (Brown, 2003, p. 160). Eventually? Why do we need to wait? Why not implement the changes right now? Changing the environment is exactly what many people opposed to stimulants have been saying for years. Faraone’s take on the aetiology of ADHD is strikingly similar to Thom Hartman’s view. Both believe that ADHD is a biological, hereditary trait (Hartmann, 1996). Where they differ is that Faraone, and other biological psychiatrists, see these children as dysfunctional, with a genetic defect in need of medication. The other group sees the children as having different genes, at one end of the spectrum, and that what is needed is a different environment (Hartmann, 1996). One purpose of the genetic studies, which the pharmaceutical companies, and the psychiatry profession, have propagated, is to imply that, because it’s genetic that drugs are needed. For instance, Faraone states: ‘Many parents are reluctant for their children to take psychotropic medication and others find it difficult to maintain the prescribed regimes. These problems are mitigated by discussing the genetic etiology of ADHD’ (Faraone, 1996, p. 598).
If you are going to acknowledge that knowing about genetics has nothing to do with treatment than you should be ready to answer the general public and politicians when they ask: Then why are you doing this research? If knowing about genetics has no benefit to the patient, then one possibility for this line of research is to justify current practices. If ADHD does not have a strong genetic influence then giving a medication would be seen as very problematic, and would call into question the entire practice of medicating children with stimulants. If I were you I would delete this line about genetics and treatment.
References suggested:
Andreasen (1984) The Broken Brain.

Paper excluded: book (not peer-reviewed).

Faraone, Science.
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Brown (2003).
New attention to ADHD genes.
Science.
Paper excluded: not peer reviewed.

Hartmann (1996).
Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.
Comments taken into consideration.
60PRStephen
Faraone
1.8You state, ‘The quality of the evidence included in this review was variable and lacked any “gold standard” because no diagnostic tests for ADHD have been developed or tested.’ I suggest you be clear what you mean by ‘gold standard.’ I think you mean a laboratory test of some sort. Although I’m probably in the minority, I think that the DSM-IV diagnosis of ADHD as made by a competent health professional is a pretty good gold standard inasmuch as it is reproducible with high reliability and has clinical implications. The inter-rater reliability of the ADHD diagnosis is not much worse than, for example, many accepted ‘gold standard’ diagnoses made by radiologists.No reference suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.3.
89PRJonathan Leo1.8Evidence Summary.
In your summary, after 22 pages of discussion about the evidence, you do not cite any direct evidence that ADHD results from a biological, hereditary defect. However, you do not come right out and acknowledge this lack of evidence. The 1998 National Institutes of Health conference was much more direct when it said, ‘there are no data to indicate that ADHD is due to a brain malfunction.’
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.14.
87PRJonathan Leo1.9‘ADHD should be considered in all age groups (children adolescents, and adults), with symptom criteria adjusted for age appropriate changes in behaviour.’ And also in 1. 6, ‘There was no evidence of a need to apply a different concept of ADHD to children and adults.’ There is an important point to be mentioned here that the NICE document ignores. Allowing adults, who can make their own decisions, to take stimulants is one matter, however it is an entirely separate matter when it comes to children. The ethical questions surrounding the use of Ritalin are becoming more significant as once-medicated children are now reaching adulthood. According to a recent survey in the LA Times, a significant number of these adults are deciding to discontinue their medication (Healy, 2006b). The LA Times article quotes a 27-year-old girl who reflects back on the years she was medicated, ‘It was kind of weirdly amazing. . ..You get excited about monotonous work, honestly. Like, translating Spanish becomes totally fun . . .The thing is, it works. But why are we forcing people to be in that position that they should like something that they wouldn’t ordinarily’ (Healy, 2006). In just three short sentences this 27-year-old girl goes right to the heart of the ethical dilemma of stimulant medication: Is it right to medicate people so that they do well in school? How is it that a lay person can go right to the heart of the issue while a committee of physicians with years of training can produce a document that ignores this key point? Why are questions like this not raised by academicians in medical journals, or by the GDG?Healy (2006) The Ritalin kids grow up: many of the ADD generation say no to meds.
LA Times.
Paper excluded: not peer reviewed; relevant to treatment not validity.
Comment taken into consideration.
40CCDavid Coghill1.9.1.1I felt that these were rather weakly described and a bit ‘fluffy’ for want of a better word. It is really saying you should use the diagnostic criteria, you should actually count the symptoms you should be clear about impairment and you should consider ADHD diagnosis in all ages. I think it just needs some re-wording to make it snappier.
Also it could benefit from starting off with a very clear and strong statement saying that the diagnostic categories of ADHD and hyperkinetic disorder are considered valid and should be used. This is a very important message to clinicians, the public, the government, the press, and so on.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.14 and 5.15.
48PRDavid Cottrell1.9.1.1Will you be able to operationally define ‘moderately clinically significant’ impairment?No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.15.2 (C) ‘How should impairment be judged?’
1CCEdmund Sonuga-BarkeGeneralThis seems a very accurate and sensible document.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
2CCRussell SchacharGeneralI read the document with great interest and think that it is a solid contribution to the ongoing debate about ADHD/HKD. Given that the document is based on a review of reviews, it is not altogether easy to judge how the summary statements were reached, but they look appropriate.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
3CCGeoff KewleyGeneralI felt the review was a reasonable summary of discussion and have nothing else to add.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
4PRMargaret AlsopGeneralWe are concerned with processes preceding and following diagnosis rather than diagnosis. The concept of ADHD is multi-faceted, therefore no individual discipline is likely to be competent to identify, assess and intervene alone. As such diagnosis becomes a mechanical feature in a holistic process involving a range of professionals. A child psychiatrist or paediatrician should normally make the formal diagnosis. However, a diagnosis should only be considered valid if it is made on the basis of evidence that a particular agency is pertinent; that agency should be involved as appropriate. Medical practitioners also have a significant role to play in diagnosis and assessment in order to rule out physical factors which may lead to the symptoms similar to those of ADHD.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.15.
5PRMargaret AlsopGeneralIf these guidelines are intended to be accessible to professionals and parents from a range of disciplines who might first identify, or have concerns about, problems that may or may not result in an ADHD diagnosis, their first efforts are likely to be of a broadly psychosocial nature (that is, behavioural/cognitive and educational interventions). Currently different professionals use different terminology to describe the phenomenon of ADHD (for example, hyperkinetic disorder, behavioural problems).
The use of different terms is not helpful to professionals, children, young people, adults or their families: therefore an attempt should be made in this document to be consistent in the use of terms that have been selected for their clarity and acceptability to a wide range of professionals.
There are significant differences, sometimes of an ideological nature between different professional groups (Cooper, 1997; Hughes, 1999; Maras & Redmayne, 1997). These differences can be exaggerated through training and practice and are often reflected in different professional perceptions and views of ADHD. Differences can sometimes result in confusion, misunderstandings and conflict and may have an adverse influence on the effectiveness of multi-disciplinary/agency working. However, there is also much common ground among professionals, especially in terms of sought after outcomes of intervention. ADHD by its very nature demands a multi- agency response and provides an opportunity for medical, educational, psychological, social care and other professionals to work together.
References suggested: Cooper (1997). Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Hughes (1999). Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.

Maras & Redmayne (1997). Asked reviewer for full reference; no response.
Comment taken into consideration.
6PRMargaret AlsopGeneralWe note that there is no reference made in relation to transition between CAMHS into the adult CMHT. There is clear evidence to indicate that a high percentage of those diagnosed with ADHD in child-hood will not have any appropriate transitional plan in place, therefore it is important that an appropriate multi-agency response for transitional arrangements are identified.
(Social Exclusion Unit – Transitions Young Adults with Complex Needs)
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see NICE guideline ‘Transition to adult services’.
12PRMargaret AlsopGeneralThe following statistics provides an overview of the numbers of children facing particular risk factors out of a total population of children in England of 12 million:
  1. In 2000, 2.7 million children lived in low income families.
  2. Up to 75,000 children may be missing from school rolls.
  3. Around ten per cent of children aged 5 to 15 have a mental disorder of sufficient severity to cause them distress or to have considerable effect on the way they live and 20% of children suffer from mental health problems.
  4. One in nine children run away from home for at least a night.
  5. One in ten families in England and Wales report incidences of domestic violence in a year.
  6. In 2000 there were 91,400 conceptions to girls aged under 20.
  7. At the end of September 2001 there were approximately 5,400 households with children in bed and breakfast accommodation.
  8. There are approximately 300,000 children with disabilities in England; 110,000 of these are severely disabled.
  9. 26,800 children and young people are on the child protection register.
  10. 58,900 children and young people are in public care.
  11. 11,000 young people aged 15–20 are in young offenders institutions.
Our concerns are, how many of these include those with ADHD or possible ADHD?
Figures released by the Children and Young People’s Unit on 6th September 2002, www​.cypu.gov.uk
No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
14PRMargaret AlsopGeneralThat the assessment, diagnosis and treatment for ADHD should be delivered throughout the lifespan, services delivery should be multi-agency, multi- model and incorporate professionals from many services such as: health, education, social care, behaviour support, parenting programmes, adult community mental health, community care, prison healthcare providers, housing, employment agencies and those within the criminal justice system. Our belief is that the term EBD (emotional and behavioural difficulties) should not be used in relation to service delivery for those already diagnosed as having ADHD. Within many services the term used for those with ADHD is described as having EBD, therefore access to a full multi-agency approach may not be forthcoming. We understand that this request may be out of the remit of the GDG and NICE but would still therefore like to request it for inclusion and consideration.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
15PRMargaret AlsopGeneralNot being qualified nor trained in medicine, I do not consider it appropriate for contributions from non- medically trained individuals to be included. I can only go on experiences as a parent-carer of a young adult (25 years old ) his having been un-medicated for the first 14 years of his life and as to how medications have now turned his life around and made him feel totally inclusive to society and not another statistic within our penal system or another fatality of drug abuse/overdose.No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
16PRMargaret AlsopGeneralIt is felt that the assessments for ADHD in children should be conducted through a ‘core diagnostic’ team, this way it is multi-agency, multi-model and will rule out/include any other underlying difficulties such as ASD, learning difficulties, dyslexia, and so on.
During my own experiences, those diagnosed with ADHD as children have received a dual diagnosis of ADHD and ASD in later adolescence or adulthood.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.15.
17CCDavid CoghillGeneralIn general I very much agree with the way that the issues are addressed and the conclusions reached. Overall this is a well-structured document and reaches some clear conclusions. I think that the sample comment given above applies to this document and that, ‘The guideline highlights throughout the document where there are gaps in the evidence to support clinical practice. Although these areas are in the main text of the document, it would be helpful if there could be an additional section at the end of each chapter with areas where further research would be helpful. This would support the research agenda and maximise resources.’No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
18CCDavid CoghillGeneralWill the appendices detailing the literature be in tabular form showing sample size, and so on? As it would be very helpful to be able to see this information.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Appendix 17.1 ‘Study characteristics – Diagnosis’.
19CCDavid CoghillGeneralI have marked up minor comments on wording and so on in the document itself.No references suggested.Comments taken into consideration.
41PRDavid CottrellGeneralI found this to be a well written and coherent account of diagnostic validity issues. Given the potentially diverse readership of NICE guidelines and the complexity of the literature I thought the language clear and the research explained well.
The questions to be addressed and the methods used are set out clearly towards the end of Section 1.3 and in 1.3. The methods are appropriate for the questions asked. My comments are largely about the use of language and presentation. I have no substantive disagreement with the case that is presented.
No references suggested.Comments taken into consideration.
49PRDavid CottrellGeneral1.2. Third paragraph, line 8 – I think this should be ‘particularly’ but the whole sentence is clumsily worded and obscures meaning.
1.4.1. Second paragraph after subheading ‘Evidence’ – the final sentence is not grammatical and again obscures meaning.
1.4.3. First paragraph after subheading ‘evidence’, line 5 – presumably ‘that on this’ not ‘this on this’.
1.5. Second paragraph, line 7 – ‘who do not ADHD’ does not make sense
1.7.1. First sentence is ungrammatical.
There are other minor typos in the document but those above have the potential to distort the meaning of the text.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Section 5.3.
61PRStephen FaraoneGeneralThe following article should be of interest to you: Faraone, S. V. (2005) The scientific foundation for understanding attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a valid psychiatric disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 1–10.References suggested: Faraone, S. V. (2005) The scientific foundation for understanding attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a valid psychiatric disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 1–10. Paper excluded: opinion paper.Comment taken into consideration.
70CCSami TimimiGeneralI wish to make the following points on the above document:
The document states in its introduction that, ‘The Guideline Development Group (GDG) acknowledged at the outset that the use of the diagnosis of ADHD has been the subject of considerable controversy and debate’ and ‘The relative lack of a validated reference standard (indicated by SIGN diagnostic study quality assessment, see Appendix A) means that the question of validity for the diagnosis of ADHD needs to draw on evidence from a wide range of sources’ [my italics]. Despite this the subsequent discussion of the evidence included no references drawn from authors who are critical of the concept of ADHD, despite the group being provided with a number of scientific reviews from such authors. The references included repeatedly cited research by a small number of researchers and research groups (including from the chair of the group) known to be supporters of the concept of ADHD. This suggests that the document lacks balance and is ideologically biased toward literature that confirms the majority of GDG members’ views.
Many members of the GDG have previously written papers or otherwise collaborated with the chair of this group. The fact that there is not one academic/practitioner who is able to represent the other side of this debate is reflected in the one-sided document the GDG has produced. It is my opinion that the conflict of interest in this group is to an extent that is unacceptable given the importance of their task.
It isn’t clear why the GDG decided to use the Washington University Diagnostic Criteria beyond the unexplained ‘ensure that a transparent, structured approach was taken’ nor is it evident whether any other systematic approach or framework was considered. However, even with these criteria, the interpretation of the GDG that the evidence they present is sufficient to support the validity of ADHD using these criteria is open to question.
No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.3 and 5.9.
77CCSami TimimiGeneralThere is clear evidence in the document that the GDG has displayed unacceptable bias in its preferred paradigm for analysing the literature, in its selection of literature and in its interpretation of the literature they selected. The fact that most of the academic members of the GDG have previously published papers with the chair of the group and that the group does not include any members with a more critical stance, strengthens the impression that the levels of bias result from conflicts of interest that are seriously unethical. The conclusions are thus not valid and could lead to serious deficiencies in practice and provide poor protection for patients, possibly exposing many more children to significant harm. The document should not be accepted. The GDG should be dismantled, a new chair appointed and a new GDG convened with a more equitable balance of opinion reflected in its membership.No references suggested.Comment addressed, see Sections 5.3 and 5.9, as well as Chapter 3, Methods.
79PRJonathan LeoGeneralTake a trait – any trait, either physical or behavioural. Given normal biological variability, if the trait is measured and subsequently plotted on a graph there will be a spectrum. Some are tall and some are short, some have long legs and some have short legs, or some have a longer attention span than others. Variability of a trait is not proof of a disease.
Take a drug’s effect. There are certain drugs that have an effect on human traits. Alli, a new diet drug, will help people lose weight – no matter what their weight to begin with. There are also drugs that have an effect on an individual’s behaviour, no matter what their behaviour to begin with. Take the stimulants, for example: response to a drug with a universal effect, like the stimulants, is not proof of a disease. (See the GDG comments page 17 Section 1.5 limitations.) These are the two most common reasons cited as evidence for a biological basis of ADHD. The dilemma for NICE is to go beyond this. As it is stands now, NICE’s conclusion that the ADHD diagnosis is valid is primarily based on the flawed premise that variability of a trait is proof of a disease. Even your own ‘Evidence Summary’, basically says it is a trait, which in your opinion should be called a disease, at one end of the spectrum. In the summary you do not (or cannot) cite a single scientific study or even an area of study confirming that ADHD is primarily a problem of biology.
If traits can be called diseases then where does this stop? A recent Op-Ed article in the New York Times addresses the problem of pathologising traits: ‘It may seem baffling, even bizarre, that ordinary shyness could assume the dimension of a mental disease. But if a youngster is reserved, the odds are high that a psychiatrist will diagnose social anxiety disorder and recommend treatment. How much credence should we give the diagnosis? Shyness is so common among American children that 42 percent exhibit it. And, according to one major study, the trait increases with age. By the time they reach college, up to 51 percent of men and 43 percent of women describe themselves as shy or introverted. Among graduate students, half of men and 48 percent of women do. Psychiatrists say that at least one in eight of these people needs medical attention’ (Lane, 23 September, 2007).
In the future will NICE have a committee deciding if ‘Is shyness a valid diagnosis?’ According to the logic of the current document that identifying a trait is somehow proof of a disease the answer would appear to be ‘yes’.
No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
90PRJonathan LeoGeneralConclusion: The NICE document provides no new insight into the diagnosis of ADHD. It has systematically ignored one side of the debate and has simply summarised the views of those involved with the ongoing medication of children. The flaws are neither subtle nor minor, nor can they be rectified with editing. The entire approach of the panel is flawed. I am not privy to the make-up of the panel but it appears that the panel had no members with a broad societal view of the ADHD diagnosis – if it did, then they were ignored. In all your discussions you seem to have one standard for biology and one for the environment. Marginal imaging studies, that compared medicated ADHD children to controls, and genetic studies, which have not found an ADHD gene, are given credence, while you cannot even cite a study linking the environment to ADHD.
The other side of the debate, that variability of a trait, and the universal effect of stimulants, are not good evidence for justifying the belief that, upwards of 10 to 15% to of the world’s children have an organic brain defect, is simply not presented. Likewise the ethics surrounding the diagnosis are ignored in the NICE document. If the NICE statement on ADHD is approved no one should be surprised when 5 years from now more British children are being prescribed stimulants. In no way should the current NICE document be considered a fair and all encompassing view of the ADHD phenomena. On the surface, it is a document couched in the language of science, but when one looks deeper at the scientific studies there is little evidence to support the disease concept of ADHD.
No references suggested.Comment taken into consideration.
Figure 1. Social class & mental health in childhood England & Wales 2004.

Figure 1

Social class & mental health in childhood England & Wales 2004. Source: Department of Health, 2004

Figure 2. Health inequality in childhood and the social geography of disadvantage.

Figure 2

Health inequality in childhood and the social geography of disadvantage.

Margaret Alsop

Executive Director, Dorset ADHD Support Group

Dr David Coghill

Senior Lecturer in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Division of Pathology and Neuroscience (Psychiatry), University of Dundee

Professor David Cottrell

Professor of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Associate Professor of Psychology, University of Leeds

Dr Stephen Faraone

Professor of Psychiatry and of Neuroscience and Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University

Dr Geoff Kewley

Consultant Paediatrician/Physician, Director of Learning Assessment and Neurocare Centre, Horsham, West Sussex

Dr Jonathan Leo

Associate Professor of Anatomy, Western University of Health Sciences, California

Dr Russell Schachar

Department of Psychiatry, Neurosciences and Mental Health Research Institute, The Hospital for Sick Children, University of Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Professor Edmund Sonuga-Barke

Professor in Psychology, Convenor of the Developmental Brain Behaviour Unit, University of Southampton

Professor Anita Thapar

Professor, Department of Psychological Medicine, Cardiff University School of Medicine

Dr Sami Timimi

Consultant Child Psychiatrist, Lincolnshire Partnership NHS Trust

Footnotes

60

References for this paper can be found at the end of the section.

61

References for this paper can be found at the end of the section.

62

References for this paper can be found at the end of the section.

63

This is a reproduction of the document that was sent to peer reviewers. As this was in draft form it contains some typographical and grammatical errors.

64

Appendices A-J referenced here are not reproduced in this publication. Please contact the authors if you would like copies of these appendices.

65

These are reproduced in Part 1 of this appendix.

Copyright © 2009, The British Psychological Society & The Royal College of Psychiatrists.

All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers. Enquiries in this regard should be directed to the British Psychological Society.

Bookshelf ID: NBK53648

Views

  • PubReader
  • Print View
  • Cite this Page
  • PDF version of this title (8.9M)

Recent Activity

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

See more...